Chaining
The clause,
not
the sentence, is the basic verbal form of statement.
When
teachers define a sentence as subject
plus
predicate, they are
really defining a clause,
and
when
they say a sentence expresses a
complete thought, they
mean
an
independent
clause asserts a propo-
sition. They are thinking of a single clause as a sentence, whereas a
sentence may comprise several clauses. Indeed, a sentence is
the
main
way
clauses are chained.
Sentences
A set of clause-statements
may
be
connected
in
three ways:
1. By making each a separate sentence
and
stringing them:
I saw Bobby's hat. It was in a tree. The wind blew
it
there. Then
it
rained.
2.
By joining several into one sentence
by
conjunctions, relative
pronouns, or punctuation:
I saw Bobby's
hat
and
it
was in a tree,
and
the wind blew
it
there,
and then
it
rained. (The famous run-on sentence of the immature
speaker.)
I saw Bobby's hat, which was in a tree, where the wind blew
it
before
it
rained.
3.
By reducing some clauses to phrases
and
embedding
them
in
others:
In
a tree I
saw
Bobby's hat, blown there
by
the wind before the rain
came.
First, learners predicate ideas separately;
then
they join
them
with
the easier conjunctions;
then
sometimes they join them
with
more difficult conjunctions
and
relative pronouns,
and
sometimes
they embed some
within
others. So
1,
2,
and
3 above represent a
growth order
if
you
keep
in
mind
that
the
difficulty of conjoining (2)
depends
on
the difficulty
of
the
connector
word
(its concept,
that
is),
and
that
the difficulty
of
embedding (3) varies considerably
with
the
kind
of
clause reduction.
To
demonstrate further
the
issue of 2
and
3, let's take another
series having a more abstract topic:
1.
Goodsayer was elected. He
adopted
the
policies advocated by
his
opponent. He
had
harshly criticized
them
when
he
was running
for office.
Notice the repetition
of
subject
and
object so clangingly present
in
children's clause strings
but
muted
here
by
the pronouns. Strings are
47
48
Chaining
uneconomical because
they
keep predicating
the
same nominals.
Personal
pronouns
disguise this,
but
of
pronouns
only
the
relative
can
solve this,
not
the
personal (he above). The
next
sentence repre-
sents
maturer
development
by
conjoining the clauses:
2. After
he
was elected, Goodsayer
adopted
the
policies
that
his
opponent
was advocating,
which
he
had
harshly
criticized
when
he
was
running
for office.
But
the
following version,
which
reduces
and
embeds four clauses
from
the
first, requires substantially more development:
3. Once elected, Goodsayer
adopted
the
policies advocated
by
his
opponent-the
very policies
he
had
harshly
criticized during the
campaign.
It
is
worth
the
trouble to
study
these three sentences
and
compare
the changes, because
the
differences exemplify a great deal about
growth
in
sentence development.
Though
shorter, the last sentence
is
harder
than
the
second
because students have to develop clauses
first before
they
can
learn
to reduce them.
Of
course, a speaker or
writer does
not
normally compare alternatives, as
we
are doing here.
Most composition is more spontaneous
than
that,
and
even
hard
revision
would
not
produce
the
shorter version
until
the
autl:_ior
had
logged considerable composing experience. Compactness comes
harder,
and
when
length is a sign oflooseness, as
in
run-on
sentences,
it
shows immaturity.
This is
not
to say
the
compacter version is always better.
It
has a
different emphasis, partly because it leaves more implicit.
It
might
not
therefore
suit
as well a given intent.
The
point
here is
that
to be
able to
reduce
clauses
and
embed
them
in
each
other,
when
this
relates concepts appropriately, indicates fairly advanced growth.
Of
course, "reducing
and
embedding clauses" is only a
manner
of speak-
ing since
no
one sees people
do
this except occasionally perhaps
in
written
revision,
but
to infer some
such
inner
process gradually
occurring seems reasonable since language users
of
different maturity
levels differ
by
just
such
sample sentences. Inserting links between
clauses is easier
than
reducing
and
fusing clauses,
but
the
conceptual
difficulty of
individual
linking
words-spatial-temporal
versus logi-
cal conjunctions, for
example-must
be
allowed
for.
As clauses are conjoined
and
embedded,
they
require certain
meta-communicative
words-conjunctions
like but,
or,
although,
because, unless or relative
pronouns
like who, which,
and
where. The
statements are
the
communication,
and
these connectors metacom-
municate
about
how
to take
and
relate
the
statements. As we said,
Chaining
49
such
words
are
harder
just as concepts,
but
they
are also
hard
because
they
relate statements to form more complex ideas. Conjunctions
name
explicitly
the
relation, whereas relative
pronouns
merely plug
one
nominal
into two predicates,
naming
nothing
and
relating im-
plicitly instead. See preceding examples.
Growth Sequence
18:
Expanding the repertory
of
clause-connecting
options as follows:
• String
of
separate independent clauses, each a sentence
• Clauses conjoined by coordinating conjunctions (and, but, or)
and time-space conjunctions
• Clauses conjoined
by
logical subordinating conjunctions and
fused by relative pronouns
• Clauses reduced and embedded in each other
Two things are
important
to
the
formulation above. One is to
emphasize
that
mature
learners
not
only
can
do
these
things
but
do
them
appropriately, according to the place
of
the
statements
in
a total
discourse. Complexity for its
own
sake is
no
mark
of
maturity. Com-
plexity is necessary
but
not
sufficient for fullest growth. A string of
single-clause sentences
can
be very effective for making
an
image or
idea
dawn
gradually
on
the
receiver. It understates
and
it
also
stretches out
the
reader's assimilation time. Mature
students
would
for these reasons
employ
such
a string
even
though
they
were capable
of fashioning very intricate
sentence
structures.
The
second
matter
is
the
critical one of subordinating concepts
one to
another
so
that
they
are related
with
the
proper
emphasis.
Stringing makes all statements equal, besides
not
making explicit
the
relations among them.
The
only
connection
is
the
primitive one
of
first-to-last,
which
says nothing
unless
the
statements are about
events,
in
which
case
the
order
of stringing is
assumed
to
be
the
order
of
their
occurrence. Coordinating conjunctions say
that
the
state-
ments
are equal
in
rank
(co-ordinate)
in
addition
to being, say alter-
native (or) or adversative (but). More properly speaking,
the
statements are equal
and
the
conjunctions are coordinating because
equality is
in
the
nature of
the
logical relationships and,
or,
and
but,
if
you
think
about it,
whereas
the
subordinating conjunctions,
such
as proviso (unless), concessions [although),
condition
(ij),
and
the
time-space conjunctions require
that
the
clause
they
introduce
be
subordinate to
the
one to
which
it
is conjoined. (Time-space clauses
50
Chaining
are always adverbial modifiers
of
course,
and
hence
subordinated
to
the
sentence
predicate.)
Now
let's bring
in
the
conventional
terms:
• Single-clause
sentence-"simple
sentence"
• Clauses conjoined
by
coordinating
conjunctions-"compound
sentence"
• Clauses conjoined
by
subordinating
conjunctions-"complex
sentence"
• Clauses conjoined
by
both
coordinating
and
subordinating con-
junctions-"
compound-complex
sentence"
Although
this
progression roughly parallels
our
growth sequence,
it
allows
neither
for
the
embedding
of
reduced
clauses
nor
for
the
variation
in
the
difficulty among conjunctions
and
between
conjunc-
tions
and
relative
pronouns.
This
old
classification
of
sentences does
bring out, however,
subordination
and
emphasis, two critical factors
of
growth
in
making sentences
and
sentence sequences
out
of
basic
statements.
From
his research
with
children's
writing Kellogg
Hunt
con-
cluded
that
sentence growth is
marked
by
(1) increasing modification
of
nouns
by
large clusters
of
adjectives, relative clauses,
and
reduced
relative clauses; (2) increasing use
of
nominalizations
other
than
nouns
and
pronouns
for subjects
and
objects (clauses, infinitival
and
gerundive constructions),
and
(3)
embedding
of
sentences to
an
in-
creasing
depth
(entailed
by
1
and
2). * A
sentence
having a single
word
or
phrase
for a subject
("
Such an
idea
never
occurred
to her.")
is easier to formulate
than
one
having a clause for a subject
("
What
other people
might
think
of
her
actions
doesn't
concern
her.").
In
the
second
example,
the
nominalization,
in
italics, is a clause
embedded
in
the
clause of
the
whole
sentence
and
containing, as its
own
subject, a
nominal
phrase
("other people") like
that
serving
in
the
first example as subject of
the
whole
sentence ("Such
an
idea").
Growth Sequence
19:
Toward increasing versatility in constructing
sentences, exploiting more nearly the total resources inherent in
modifying, conjoining, reducing, and
embedding
clauses; and
toward increasing comprehension
of
sentences
of
such range.
* Kellogg Hunt, Grammatical Structures Written
at
Three Grade Levels, National
Council
of
Teachers
of
English, Champaign, IL, 1965
Chaining
51
Syllogisms
A special case of conjoining clauses was
touched
on
when
we
spoke
of conditional tenses joined
by
if.
When
two or more conditional
clauses are
linked
to each other
and
to a conclusion clause, a syllo-
gism is created.
"If
high
spending
contributes to inflation,
and
if
advertising
and
credit stimulate high spending,
then
advertising
and
credit contribute to inflation."
At
the
material level,
such
a conjunction of conditions
may
be
stated
in
a sentence like this:
"If
heavy
rain
falls a long
time
on
loose
dirt,
and
if
the
terrain is steeply tilted, a
mudslide
will
occur." Note
that
this
logical relationship
may
be
expressed
by
other conjunctions
and
by
adverbs: "A
mudslide
occurs because heavy
rain
falls a long
time
on
loose
dirt
and
because
the
terrain is steeply tilted." Or:
"The
rain
falls a long time
on
loose dirt,
and
the
terrain is steeply tilted;
so
[therefore] a
mudslide
occurs."
The
point
is
that
underneath
these
various conjunctions
and
adverbs there lies a single logical relation-
ship. This relationship is called
entailment-certain
things being so
entail other things being so. (See
on
page
43
Susanne Langer's men-
tion
of entailment.)
It
is
important
to realize
that
what
is
the
same
at
the
conceptual
level-entailment-may
be
expressed at
the
verbal
level as causality, conditionality, or something else.
Syllogizing
may
be, first of all,
implicit
or explicit and, second,
may
take several forms.
It
is
an
important
sort of logical growth to
look for,
but
the
teacher
can
expect
it
to be revealed
in
more
than
one
verbal way,
if
made
explicit at all. A syllogism
may
perfectly well
exist
in
a discourse
without
being verbalized
in
a single sentence. It
may
be
embodied
in
another
kind
of linguistic linking
than
conjoined
clauses-in
one of
the
other kinds
of
chaining discussed next.
Transitional words
Besides conjunctions
and
relative pronouns, certain adverbs connect
clauses
and
do so explicitly as conjunctions (moreover, however,
nevertheless, so, therefore, accordingly,
and
others referring to ideas
in
previous clauses),
but
these differ
in
being situated within a clause,
not
between
clauses, so
that
they tie clauses together only
by
throw-
ing
an
idea
bridge,
not
by
connecting grammatically. These are
what
we
might call transition words, because
they
are
added
to a clause to
relate statements explicitly
in
the
same
way
that
whole sentences
may
be
stuck into a discourse to effect transitions from one
main
idea
or
part
of
the
organization to another ("Leaving aside for
the
moment
the objections to
this
idea, let's
now
turn
to
....
").
52
Chaining
Transitions, too, constitute
meta-communication
and
hence
do
not
occur to speakers or writers too egocentric to realize
that
an
audience
might
not
know
how
to
connect
their
clauses
unless
guided.
On
the
other
hand,
mature
communicators may choose to
omit
some transitions as being
unnecessary
, heavy, or verbose for
the
ideas
and
the
audience
involved, or
may
wish
to
speak
implicitly to
make
their
audience
think
more
and
work
out
connections for it-
self-obviously
a sophisticated stance,
indeed
a very confident one.
And
once again,
the
presence of
the
words-hence
or
so,
say-does
not
guarantee
the
presence of
the
concepts they
stand
for.
A trick of
weak writers is to plaster
their
composition together
with
there/ores
and
moreovers
in
lieu of thought.
Punctuation
Colons, semicolons,
and
sometimes commas also
connect
state-
ments.
They
are
much
less explicit
than
word
connectors,
but
they
have some meaning. A colon
tends
to act as
an
equation
mark
and
hence
assumes one meaning of to be (identity),
and
a semicolon or
comma
implies
unusual
closeness
between
clause-statements. With-
out
indicating the nature of the relation,
this
binding
nevertheless
invites readers to
supply
for themselves a conjunction of time, cau-
sality,
contradiction
,
and
so
on,
according
to
context.
Paragraphing
Paragraphing is another
way
of
implying
relations
between
state-
ments. A paragraph break, for example,
between
one statement
and
another means
that
the
thought
takes a bigger
jump
than
is
usual
between
sentences or
that
thought
is shifting to another time or
plane
or domain. Placing one
statement
at
the
beginning of a paragraph
and
another
within
may
mean
that
the
first is
superordinate
or more
general
and
that
the
next one is subordinate or more concrete. The
first sentence
might
state a generality
and
the
second
state
an
in-
stance or consequence
of
it.
The
relative positioning
may
obviate
the
need
of
"for example" or "so." The sheer
order
in
which
statements
are
chained
means
something of course, since juggling the order
would
usually
make considerable difference
in
the
intelligibility of
the
message. Paragraphing imposes
upon
this sequence other pat-
terns
of
significance
by
clumping
together statements so
that
dis-
tance, salience,
and
subordination
vary among
them
and
hence
imply
certain interrelations.
The
ways of chaining sentences
that
comprise
paragraphs
can
comprise the organization of
an
entire discourse.
Chaining
53
Organization
The
possibilities
of
paragraphing are the possibilities
of
organizing a
whole
discourse.
The
continuity
may
vary
in
length,
but
once
beyond
the
sentence (with its special grammatical rules
of
relating)
the
ways
of
chaining statements are
the
same as for composing
the
units
of
any
other
linear
medium-serial
order, juxtaposition,
and
pattern.
These
are universal factors
of
form
and
constitute
what
English teachers
mean
by
"organization"
in
a composition. Form establishes relations
by
sheer
selection
and
arrangement,
without
naming relations. Form
speaks-but
implicitly. So clause-connecting
throughout
an
entire
continuity
of statements is
nothing
less
than
the
overall form
of
a
complete discourse,
and
the forms
with
which
people
compose
dis-
courses are general forms
common
to
many
other media.
Ascending and descending forms In music,
we
speak
of
the
first
statement
of
a
theme
and
of its later variations. This form compares
to
an
opening
statement
of
the
main
idea
of
a discourse followed
by
the
elaborating
of
its
implication
in
substatements. Either a
whole
discourse, a subdivision of it, a paragraph, or even a
sentence
could
be organized
this
way-from
higher
to lower abstraction.
It
is
the
deductive form exemplified
by
the
famous
"topic
sentence,"
which
sets a frame
within
which
details, implications, consequences,
evidence,
and
so
on
are
then
expounded.
Within a
sentence
this
works
out
as a
main
clause followed
by
subordinate clauses
and
by
modifiers:
They just had
to
peer over the rim, although the canyon terrified
them, leaning
far
forward over planted
feet,
heads tipped back
for
balance, eyes turned down their cheeks.
Within a
whole
discourse, paragraphs
would
so descend.
The
opposite form
may
be equally right,
depending
on
intent
and
content.
It
is
the
inductive
order,
by
which
a
theme
is gradually
built
up
through
partial statements
until
arrived at climactically.
Within
a
sentence, modifiers
and
subordinate clauses
would
prepare
for
the
main
clause,
which
would
come
at
the
end
as climax (the so-called
periodic sentence).
Whenever someone asked her
to
sing once again, perhaps
at
tea time
in the old sunroom, perhaps at a garden gathering in the morning,
imploring, saying she had
no
right
to
withhold that
gift,
her plump
hand would
go
to
her throat, and her head would slowly
wag
no.
Following
the
same
model
on
larger scales, a paragraph or a
whole
discourse
would
start
low
and
build
high, suspensefully, revealing
only
enough
per
statement
to carry
the
receiver to
the
next,
broader
54
Chaining
view,
whether
the increments are
physical
details of a complex ob-
ject, causes
of
some effect, or arguments leading
toward
a conclusion.
Various orders The direction
that
the
chaining moves
between
low
and
high
abstraction,
whole
and
part, generality
and
instance, is
of
great significance for
composition
and
comprehension, for
the
op-
posed
approaches orient
the
receiver very differently.
The
growing
learner has to
understand
that
these options exist
and
what
effects
they
have. Chaining
need
not
follow
the
order
in
which
events,
images, or thoughts originally or logically occur, because rhetorical
ends
must
be served. A reader
may
see a
scene
more clearly
if
the
writer starts
with
a
panoramic
shot
and
then
zooms
in
on
details,
but
like William
Faulkner
and
Stephen
Crane
on
occasion,
the
writer
may
want
the
reader to experience
with
the character
the
feeling,
precisely, of
not
being
on
top of a situation.
An
effect of dawning,
produced
in
many
poems, comes from forcing
the
receivers to orient
themselves
by
minimal
cues
that
imply
perhaps
several possibilities
that
must
be
considered
and
checked
out
as
the
statement
continuity
proceeds. A logical
conclusion
might
go
either
at
the
beginning or
at
the
end
of a discourse,
depending
on
whether
the
reader's knowing
the
conclusion
first makes following
the
arguments
much
easier or
on
whether
the
writer
wishes
readers to
work
through
in
their
own
minds
the
steps
by
which
the
conclusion
was reached.
It
may
be better to derange
the
order
in
which
events occurred
and
start
in
the
middle,
as
Homer
did
with
the
Iliad,
then
flash back
to
the
beginning, or to
cut
back
and
forth among different periods, as
Marcel Proust
and
Kurt Vonnegut do,
in
order to juxtapose events
in
a new,
mental
relation. Inductive
and
deductive
orders
may
be
com-
bined
as
when
a
main
statement is
built
up
by
evidence
then,
once
established
and
warranted,
applied
to various domains to see
what
it
will
turn
up. Repetition is also
an
important
formal device
common
to
both
writing
and
music
as
the
"motif."
Growth Sequence
20:
Toward using and responding to the full
rhetorical possibilities for chaining statements
by
grammar, transi-
tional words, punctuation, paragraphing, and organizational form,
according to the commitment
of
the whole discourse,
Emphasis
must
be
on
good
judgment
in
playing options. No
particular
sentence
construction, paragraph structure, or organiza-
tional form
is
better
than
another except relative to
the
communica-
tion
needs
of
the
content
and
intent. Growth does
not
consist
of
merely
acquiring
the
tools
of
metacommunication
to
name
or state
connections explicitly. These tools constitute
the
technical prereq-
Chaining
55
uisite
but
alone are
not
enough. Always,
the
learner
must
learn to
judge, as either sender or receiver, if metacommunication is desir-
able. Too often teachers incline to value only the explicit, because
they
can
see
it
and
thereby
know
what
a student's thought is,
but
explicitness is definitely only half
of
the
matter. Since
not
all
can
ever be said, discoursing is always a matter
of
ascertaining
how
much
will do
the
trick properly.
A concept may play different roles
in
a complex of concepts, may
be
more or less conscious
in
the
speaker, may
be
more or less explicit
in
a discourse,
and
so may for these reasons
be
conveyed by a single
word, a phrase, a simple sentence, a complex sentence, a continuity
of sentences, a metaphor, a motif, or a formal
pattern
in
the organi-
zation of the total work. A learner grows
in
mastery of composing
and
comprehending these alternatives for matching thought
with
speech.