Washington University in St. Louis Washington University in St. Louis
Washington University Open Scholarship Washington University Open Scholarship
Olin Business School Electronic Theses and
Dissertations
Washington University Open Scholarship
Summer 5-15-2023
Manage Your Money Wisely: How Consumers and Marketers Can Manage Your Money Wisely: How Consumers and Marketers Can
Effectively Communicate Money Issues Effectively Communicate Money Issues
Alexander Park
Washington University in St. Louis
Follow this and additional works at: https://openscholarship.wustl.edu/olin_etds
Part of the Advertising and Promotion Management Commons, Behavioral Neurobiology Commons,
Marketing Commons, and the Social and Behavioral Sciences Commons
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Park, Alexander, "Manage Your Money Wisely: How Consumers and Marketers Can Effectively
Communicate Money Issues" (2023).
Olin Business School Electronic Theses and Dissertations
. 25.
https://openscholarship.wustl.edu/olin_etds/25
This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Washington University Open Scholarship at
Washington University Open Scholarship. It has been accepted for inclusion in Olin Business School Electronic
Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Washington University Open Scholarship. For more
information, please contact [email protected].
WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY IN ST. LOUIS
Olin Business School
Dissertation Examination Committee:
Cynthia Cryder, Chair
Rachel Gershon
Robyn LeBoeuf
Hannah Perfecto
Sydney Scott
Marissa Sharif
Manage Your Money Wisely:
How Consumers and Marketers Can Effectively Communicate Money Issues
by
Alexander B. Park
A dissertation presented to
Olin Business School
of Washington University in
partial fulfillment of the
requirements for the degree
of Doctor of Philosophy in Business Administration
May 2023
St. Louis, Missouri
© 2023, Alexander B. Park
ii
Table of Contents
List of Figures .............................................................................................................................v
List of Tables ............................................................................................................................ vi
Acknowledgements .................................................................................................................. vii
Abstract .................................................................................................................................. viii
1.1 Introduction ..................................................................................................................1
1.2 Conceptual Background ................................................................................................3
1.2.1 The Socio-psychological Complexity of Money ................................................................ 3
1.2.2 Social Connection Strength ............................................................................................... 5
1.2.3 Selecting the Approach Social Richness Matters ............................................................ 7
1.2.4 Overview of Studies ....................................................................................................... 10
1.3 Study 1A ..................................................................................................................... 11
1.3.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 11
1.3.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 12
1.4 Study 1B ..................................................................................................................... 14
1.4.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 14
1.4.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 15
1.4.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 17
1.5 Study 2........................................................................................................................ 18
1.5.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 18
1.5.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 19
1.5.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 20
1.6 Study 3........................................................................................................................ 21
1.6.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 21
1.6.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 22
1.6.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 24
1.7 Study 4A ..................................................................................................................... 24
1.7.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 25
1.7.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 26
1.7.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 27
1.8 Study 4B ..................................................................................................................... 28
1.8.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 28
1.8.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 29
1.8.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 30
iii
1.9 Study 5........................................................................................................................ 30
1.9.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 31
1.9.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 32
1.9.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 35
1.10 General Discussion ..................................................................................................... 35
2.1 Introduction ................................................................................................................ 38
2.2 Conceptual Background .............................................................................................. 41
2.2.1 Corporate Social Responsibility: A Double-Edged Sword ............................................... 41
2.2.2 Commitment and Charitable Credit ................................................................................. 42
2.2.3 Periodic Donations on Commitment and Charitable Perceptions ..................................... 43
2.2.4 Total Donation Size Matters ........................................................................................... 45
2.2.5 Overview of Studies ....................................................................................................... 47
2.3 Study 1A ..................................................................................................................... 48
2.3.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 48
2.3.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 49
2.4 Study 1B ..................................................................................................................... 50
2.4.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 50
2.4.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 51
2.4.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 52
2.5 Study 2........................................................................................................................ 52
2.5.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 53
2.5.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 54
2.5.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 55
2.6 Study 3........................................................................................................................ 55
2.6.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 55
2.6.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 56
2.6.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 58
2.7 Study 4........................................................................................................................ 58
2.7.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 58
2.7.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 60
2.7.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 61
2.8 Study 5........................................................................................................................ 62
2.8.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 63
2.8.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 64
2.8.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 65
iv
2.9 Study 6........................................................................................................................ 66
2.9.1 Methods ......................................................................................................................... 66
2.9.2 Results ........................................................................................................................... 67
2.9.3 Discussion ...................................................................................................................... 68
2.10 General Discussion ..................................................................................................... 69
Appendix .................................................................................................................................. 70
3.1.1 Chapter 1: Appendix A ............................................................................................... 70
3.1.2 Chapter 1: Appendix B ................................................................................................ 78
3.2.1 Chapter 2: Appendix C ................................................................................................ 83
3.2.2 Chapter 2: Appendix D ............................................................................................... 89
References/Bibliography/Works Cited .................................................................................... 100
v
List of Figures
Figure 1.1: Perceived Discomfort of Discussing Sensitive Topics ...............................................5
Figure 1.2: Study 3 Mediation Model ........................................................................................ 24
Figure 1.3: Study 4A Results ..................................................................................................... 27
Figure 1.4: Study 4B Results ..................................................................................................... 30
Figure 2.1: Study 1A Stimuli ..................................................................................................... 49
Figure 2.2: Study 1B Stimuli ..................................................................................................... 51
Figure 2.3: Study 3 Mediation Model ........................................................................................ 57
Figure 2.4: Study 4 Mediation Models ...................................................................................... 61
Figure 2.5: Study 5 Results ....................................................................................................... 65
Figure 2.6: Study 6 Results ....................................................................................................... 68
vi
List of Tables
Table 1.1: Hypotheses ............................................................................................................... 10
Table 1.2: Study 1A Results ...................................................................................................... 13
Table 1.3: Study 1B Results ...................................................................................................... 15
Table 1.4: Study 2 Results ......................................................................................................... 20
Table 1.5: Study 5 Results ......................................................................................................... 32
Table 2.1: Study 1B Results ...................................................................................................... 51
Table 2.2: Study 2 Results ......................................................................................................... 54
vii
Acknowledgments
First, I would like to express my deepest gratitude to the best advisor anyone could ask
for, Cynthia Cryder. Thank you, Cindy, for showing me what it means to be a phenomenal
scholar and mentor. I am forever grateful for your continuous inspiration, wisdom, and emotional
support. To Robyn LeBoeuf, Sydney Scott, and Hannah Perfecto, thank you all so much for your
invaluable advice and time throughout the doctoral program. My academic journey would not
have been possible without your support and patience. To Rachel Gershon and Marissa Sharif,
while the struggle has been so real, your unwavering encouragement and mentorship have made
the experience the most exciting, laughter-filled, and happy struggle one could ask for.
Second, I would like to thank the Consumer Behavior Decision Science Lab and the
Marketing Department at WashU for their endless generosity and encouragement. Moreover, I
would like to thank my fellow Consumer Behavior cohorts for their friendship and for being
such a wonderful group.
Finally, I would like to thank my amazing family and friends for their support and
encouragement. Thank you all for believing in me.
Alexander B. Park
Washington University in St. Louis
May 2023
viii
ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION
Manage Your Money Wisely:
How Consumers and Marketers Can Effectively Communicate Money Issues
by
Alexander Park
Doctor of Philosophy in Business Administration
Washington University in St. Louis, 2023
Professor Cynthia Cryder, Chair
Consumers and marketers find it challenging to communicate money matters. For
example, consumers experience discomfort and uneasiness when discussing financial issues with
their social relationships. Firms often receive backlash when sharing their good financial deeds
(e.g., charitable efforts) with consumers. Because financial matters can be a sensitive topic, in
my research, I explore the difficulties consumers and marketers experience when communicating
money issues and how they can better navigate these problems.
In Chapter One, I investigate a particularly uneasy interaction that consumers often face
with their friends and acquaintances: the need to ask for money back. Seven fully preregistered
studies (N = 5,543) show that consumers’ approach to resolving peer debt varies based on their
closeness with the requestee. Specifically, consumers prefer communication methods low in
social richness (e.g., digital apps) when requesting money back from weak social connections
such as distant acquaintances. However, they prefer communication methods high in social
richness (e.g., in-person interactions) when requesting money back from strong social
connections such as close friends. Process evidence shows that this pattern occurs because 1)
ix
consumers anticipate discomfort when requesting money back from distant acquaintances in
person, driving them away from in-person requests and toward digital apps, and 2) consumers
are more averse to appearing impersonal with close friends, driving them away from digital apps
and toward in-person requests. In sum, consumers adaptively approach financial interactions
based on the relationship dynamics at hand.
In Chapter Two, I investigate a domain where firms and marketers encounter issues
communicating their finances with consumers: sharing their corporate social responsibility
(CSR) activities. CSR is essential for a firm’s brand image and financial success, but consumers
often doubt whether these engagements are self-interested marketing strategies driven by profit.
Then, how might firms effectively communicate their CSR activities? Across seven preregistered
experiments (N = 147,996; two field experiments and five lab experiments), we find that
donations presented as a series of periodic donations (e.g., $1000 donation per month for 12
months) improve company favorability more so than equivalent donation pledges presented in an
aggregate frame (e.g., a donation of $12,000). We further show that the effectiveness of this
temporal framing is driven by the perceived commitment towards the cause when a company
frames its donations periodically, consumers perceive the company to be more committed to
supporting the cause, increasing company favorability.
1
Chapter 1: Fighting Fiscal Awkwardness: To
Resolve Peer Indebtedness, Consumers
Adapt Based on Relationship Strength
1.1 Introduction
Suppose that you recently went to lunch with a co-worker. Your co-worker forgot their
wallet, so you covered their meal. Several days passed, and this co-worker still has not repaid
you, so you decide to ask for the money back. Would you make this request in person? Or would
you avoid the in-person confrontation and send a digital request instead?
Consumers often lend and borrow money from each other (Banerjee and Duflo 2007; Lee
and Persson 2016; Morvinski and Shani 2022; Rona-Tas and Guseva 2018). Although such
exchanges are frequent, navigating interpersonal debt can evoke unease because of money's
social and psychological complexities (Belk and Wallendorf 1990; Sun and Slepian 2020;
Zelizer 1989). Money can represent power (Zelizer 1989), status (Ivanic, Overbeck, and Nunes
2011), and achievement (Ridgeway 2014; Rose and Orr 2007). As such, thoughts about money
can trigger anxiety (Fitch et al. 2011; Sweet et al. 2013), distrust (Yamauchi and Templer 1982),
and even psychological pain (Prelec and Loewenstein 1998; Zellermayer 1996). Given the
complex social dynamics surrounding money, how do consumers navigate financial matters in
social contexts?
The current research investigates how consumers request owed money back from peers,
finding that consumers strategically tailor their approach to fit the relationship in question.
2
Specifically, we propose and find that controlling for how frequently consumers see the
requestee, the strength of consumers’ social connection with each other affects request behaviors.
We show that because consumers anticipate substantial discomfort when requesting owed money
from weak social connections, they request in ways that avoid face-to-face interactions (e.g.,
digitally). However, in the case of strong social connections, there is greater concern that a
request for repayment could appear impersonal. Therefore, with strong social ties, consumers
request via socially rich means (e.g., in-person) to avoid appearing impersonal to the requestee.
This research makes several contributions to the literature on financial decision-making,
social relationships, and consumers’ decisions in an expanding digital landscape. We examine
the important, yet understudied, domain of peer indebtedness. While informal loans are
commonplace among peers and households (e.g., Banerjee and Duflo 2007), little is understood
about how consumers navigate owing and recouping money from peers. Second, this research
contributes to the literature examining the intersection of social relationships and personal
finance. Despite the known perils of mixing money and relationships (Clark and Mills 1979,
2012; Fiske 1992), the two are frequently and inevitably intertwined (Corfman and Lehmann
1987; Ferber and Lee 1974; Simpson, Griskevicius, and Rothman 2012). This intersection of
money and relationships has been studied most extensively within romantic relationships
(Garbinsky and Gladstone 2019; Olson and Rick 2021; Rick, Small, and Finkel 2011); however,
there is limited work outside the romantic realm. Finally, we contribute to the growing literature
seeking to understand consumer finance in the digital world. Some recent work documents how
digital payment apps affect consumption experiences (e.g., Huang et al. 2020). However, to our
knowledge, we are the first to examine when and why digital payment apps are used for their
primary purpose resolving peer debt.
3
The following sections develop a theoretical basis for why and when peer indebtedness
elicits discomfort. Relying on this theoretical foundation, we argue that request methods low in
social richness, such as digital apps, can minimize discomfort for interactions among distant
acquaintances in particular. By contrast, in the case of close personal relationships, the risk of
appearing impersonal becomes a dominant consideration, driving consumers away from digital
apps and toward options higher in social richness, including in-person requests. In short, we
propose that consumers adjust financial interactions in social contexts based on the relationship
priorities at hand.
1.2 Conceptual Background
1.2.1 The Socio-psychological Complexity of Money
Consumers associate money with status, achievement, security, and power (Furnham,
Wilson, and Telford 2012; Rose and Orr 2007). Depending on how it is used, money can be so
exalted that it is viewed as sacred (Baker and Jimmerson 1992; Belk and Wallendorf 1990;
McGraw, Tetlock, and Kristel 2003; Zelizer 1989). Possessions acquired with money can
become important reflections of consumers’ identities (Belk 1988), and accordingly, consumers
often perceive a “rich self” to be a “happy self” (Aknin, Norton, and Dunn 2009; Kahneman and
Deaton 2010; Luft 1957; Mogilner 2010; Mogilner and Norton 2016).
At the same time, money serves as a source of great insecurity and angst (Furnham 1984;
Furnham, Wilson, and Telford 2012; Rose and Orr 2007). Consumers perceive money to be a
signal of one’s competence and abilities (Furnham 1984), and therefore, indebtedness or a
perceived lack of money is associated with lower psychological and even physiological well-
being (Clayton et al. 2015; Fitch et al. 2011; Ong, Theseira, and Ng 2019; Rose and Orr 2007;
4
Sweet et al. 2013). Because money, or lack thereof, can trigger insecurities, discussing one’s
finances with others may elicit an array of negative emotions, including discomfort and
nervousness, ultimately making it one of the most avoided conversational topics in many cultures
(Belk and Wallendorf 1990; Hart, VanEpps, and Schweitzer 2021; Krueger 1991; Sun and
Slepian 2020; Trachtman 1999; Wherry, Seefeldt, and Alvarez 2019).
To explore this notion, we collected data on the discomfort of financial discussions
relative to other sensitive topics. A pilot study (N = 544) measured the discomfort of discussing
ten sensitive topics, including money issues, sex, relationship problems, and politics, with both
close friends and distant acquaintances. We found that “money issues” was rated as the second
most uncomfortable topic to discuss with these social connections (see Figure 1.1; for pilot study
details, see Appendix A). These results are broadly consistent with those from Sun and Slepian
(2020), showing that money and financial matters were the most commonly avoided discussion
topics, and similarly, Hart, VanEpps, and Scwheitzer (2021), finding that when individuals are
asked to generate highly sensitive questions, they often produce questions about money. We
further found that respondents consistently rated these topics as substantially more
uncomfortable to discuss with distant acquaintances relative to close friends. Given these results
and the prevalence of peer indebtedness, it is a high priority to understand how consumers can
better manage their fiscal discomfort with their social networks.
FIGURE 1.1: PERCEIVED DISCOMFORT OF DISCUSSING SENSITIVE TOPICS
WITH PEERS
5
NOTE. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.
1.2.2 Social Connection Strength
We posit that social connection strength has an important influence on how consumers
approach financial matters with peers. Greater emotional intimacy and feelings of connectedness
between individuals dictate the strength of a social connection (Aron, Aron, Smollan 1992;
Granovetter 1973). Relationships such as those with family members, close friends, and
significant others tend to be characterized as strong social connections. Consumers have had
numerous interactions, shared experiences, and feelings of affection with these strong social ties;
thus, they tend to share a strong bond (Garcia-Rada et al. 2021; Krachardt, Nohria, and Eccles
2003). Conversely, relationships with acquaintances and distant others, defined as weak social
ties, tend to be less developed. There is a relative lack of emotional closeness, feelings of
connectedness, and importance attached to these relationships (Granovetter 1973; Lydon,
Jamieson, and Holmes 1997). Within this conceptualization, it is important to note that weak
6
social ties are not strangers nor disliked individuals, nor even necessarily mere transactional
connections (Clark and Mills 1993, 2012); rather, both strong and weak social ties generally refer
to positive social relationships, differing primarily in psychological closeness (Fowler and
Christakis 2008; Granovetter 1973).
Consumers’ peer networks consist of both strong (i.e., close friends) and weak social
connections (i.e., acquaintances). We propose that consumers have distinct approaches to
navigating financial interactions with each relationship type. Specifically, we argue that the
relationship priorities determine how consumers approach financial interactions.
An important feature of weak social ties is a lack of a close personal connection (Lydon,
Jamieson, and Holmes 1997). The lower levels of intimacy and emotional connection between
individuals with weak social ties can elicit greater discomfort and awkwardness during in-person
interactions, particularly when the topic is fraught. This is evident in the pilot study results,
where participants consistently rated awkward topics to be considerably more uncomfortable to
discuss with a distant acquaintance than a close friend (Figure 1.1). We predict that consumers
anticipate greater levels of discomfort when requesting owed money in person from weak social
ties (see Table 1.1, Hypothesis 2).
Although sensitive topics can evoke strong discomfort from weak social connections,
consumers may be less concerned about discomfort arising in interactions with close social
connections, consistent with our pilot study results (Figure 1.1). Consumers have a close
personal bond with their strong social connections. At the same time, these stronger bonds also
indicate that consumers value these relationships and might feel a need to signal care. Consumers
are generally more concerned about how their strong social connections view them (Garcia-Rada
7
et al. 2021; Wilcox and Stephen 2014), and consequently, strong social ties exert an especially
powerful influence on consumer behavior (Brown and Reingen 1987). For instance, consumers
avoid getting impersonal gifts for their close friends, sometimes even prioritizing gifts that signal
their personal relationship over prioritizing the recipient’s gift preferences (Ward and
Broniarczyk 2016). Greater concern for maintaining close relationships suggests that consumers
might be especially averse to appearing impersonal with this group in order to protect cherished
connections (see Table 1.1, Hypothesis 3).
1.2.3 Selecting the Approach Social Richness Matters
As outlined above, we predict that 1) consumers anticipate greater discomfort when
interacting with weak social connections regarding financial matters, and 2) consumers are more
averse to appearing impersonal with strong social connections. Due to these forces, we propose
that consumers use approaches differing in social richness to resolve peer indebtedness with
these two groups.
Social richness is defined as the degree to which a medium of communication varies in
intimacy and immediacy (Daft and Lengel 1986; Short, Williams, and Christie, 1976). Under this
theorization, face-to-face interactions are high in social richness because they are interpersonal
and synchronous, allowing for various social cues, immediate feedback, and personal
connection. On the other hand, digital interactions are low in social richness due to their indirect
communication and asynchronous features, rendering limited social cues, a delayed response,
and impersonality.
A second pilot test (N = 100) gauged the social richness of seven different approaches for
communicating about financial matters with peers: 1) in person, 2) phone calls, 3) texts, 4)
8
emails, 5) Venmo, 6) PayPal, and 7) a standard bank app (for pilot study details, see Appendix
A). Based on conceptualizations of social richness (Daft and Lengel 1986; Short, Williams, and
Christie, 1976), participants answered three questions: 1) “To what extent is [communication
method] a socially rich way to communicate with others?”, 2) “To what extent would you feel
that you are in the presence of others when communicating [communication method]?”, and 3)
“To what extent would you feel connected with others when communicating [communication
method]” on a 11-point Likert scale (1 = Not at all, 11 = Extremely; α = .94). The order in which
participants saw the seven communication methods was randomized. Consistent with prior
theories of social richness, post-hoc comparisons showed that participants rated in-person
interactions as the most socially rich, followed by phone calls, texts, emails, Venmo, PayPal, and
then a standard bank app (see Appendix A). In the current research, we focus the bulk of our
investigations on the communication media that are the highest on this social richness dimension
(in-person requests) versus the lowest (digital payment apps). Moreover, we focus on how these
communication media are used for financial discussions in particular.
Because digital apps are judged as low in social richness, they might allow consumers to
minimize the awkwardness inherent in resolving peer indebtedness. Indeed, one proposed benefit
of digital apps is that they can smooth some of the friction of indebtedness by allowing
consumers to avoid direct fiscal confrontation when requesting money (PYMNTS 2017). Digital
apps are often used to split bills, request or repay money among peers, and even pay vendors for
purchases (Unger et al. 2020; Zhang et al. 2017). While consumers frequently use digital apps to
request money because of convenience, we propose that this communication method can also
minimize the social discomfort inherent in fiscal interactions. Thus, we hypothesize that
consumers prefer less socially rich request methods when requesting owed money from weak
9
social ties because they expect substantial discomfort from discussing financial topics with
distant social connections.
In contrast, we predict that consumers will rely more heavily on socially rich methods,
such as in-person requests, to resolve peer indebtedness with close social connections. Digital
communication, unlike face-to-face interactions, lacks social cues and reduces personalization
(Kiesler, Siegel, and McGuire 1984; Sproull and Kiesler 1986; Walther 1995, 1996). The lack of
social features in digital communication has potential negative downstream consequences, such
as miscommunication (Kruger et al. 2005), the risk of appearing impersonal (Sproull and Kiesler
1986), or even dehumanizing the communicator (Schroeder, Kardas, and Epley 2017; Walther
1996). Drawing on these findings, we expect that for interpersonal contexts such as peer-to-peer
indebtedness, the use of digital apps poses a concern that the requester might appear impersonal
to the requestee, and this concern is greater for strong social ties. Thus, we hypothesize that
consumers prefer socially rich options, such as in-person requests, when requesting owed money
from strong social connections.
Based on the conceptualization outlined earlier regarding the key psychological dynamics
at play in social-financial interactions and insights from our two pilot tests, we propose three
main hypotheses for how consumers approach interpersonal indebtedness with strong and weak
social ties (see Table 1.1).
TABLE 1.1: HYPOTHESES
Studies
When consumers request owed money from weak social
ties, they prefer media low in social richness (e.g., digital
apps). When they request from strong social ties, they
prefer media high in social richness (e.g., in-person
1A, 1B, 2, & 5
10
Studies
requests).
Consumers anticipate more discomfort when requesting
money in person from weak versus strong social ties,
which in turn increases requests low in social richness.
3 (mediation) &
4A (moderation)
Consumers are more averse to appearing impersonal with
strong versus weak social ties, which in turn increases
requests high in social richness.
3 (mediation) &
4B (moderation)
1.2.4 Overview of Studies
We test these hypotheses across seven preregistered studies. Studies 1A and 1B use
retrospective recall paradigms to provide behavioral evidence that consumers adjust the way they
request owed money depending on the strength of their social connection with the requestee.
Study 2 replicates these findings across a variety of social scenarios while relying on
experimental manipulation of social connection strength, carefully controlling for potential
confounds. Study 3 relies on mediation to measure and test the proposed mechanisms. Studies
4A and 4B directly manipulate discomfort and impersonality, respectively. Finally, Study 5
broadens the investigation to explore how consumers resolve indebtedness with non-friend or
acquaintance relationships (i.e., a store vendor) and examines consumer choice when given the
option not to request the money back at all.
We report all conditions, data exclusions (if any), and measures for each study (Simmons,
Nelson, and Simonsohn 2012). Preregistrations for all studies as well as study materials,
including complete stimuli, measures, data, and code can be found here:
https://researchbox.org/144&PEER_REVIEW_passcode=TQPTF.
11
1.3 Study 1A: Housemate Retrospective Study
Study 1A tests H1 that consumers prefer requesting money via methods low in social
richness with weak social connections and methods high in social richness with strong social
connections. We used a retrospective paradigm with a large sample of consumers to study real
monetary requests. Given that the frequency with which consumers see a requestee may
influence how requests are made, Study 1A relied on a natural setting, house or apartment
sharing, where consumers see the other person frequently. We expect that participants who
report weaker social connections with their roommates would be more likely to request owed
money via digital apps than those who report stronger connections, while participants who report
stronger connections with their roommates would be more likely to request in person.
1.3.1 Methods
We recruited 1198 Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) participants via CloudResearch
between the ages of 18 and 40 (M
Age
= 30.33, 48.16% female). After excluding participants who
failed our preregistered exclusion criteria (described below), a final sample of 803 participants
remained. The preregistration for this study is here: https://aspredicted.org/SRV_FPX.
Participants first answered two yes or no questions about whether 1) within the past
five years, they had a housemate or roommate who was not a family member, and 2) they had
ever been in a situation where a roommate owed them money and they requested the money
back. Participants who answered yes to both questions were eligible for the study and typed
the first name or initials of the roommate from whom they had requested money back.
Participants then answered our main dependent variable question how they requested the owed
funds from their roommates among six choice options: 1) in-person (face-to-face), 2) over a
12
phone call, 3) via text message, 4) via a payment app such as Venmo, Zelle, PayPal, or Facebook
Messenger, 5) email or 6) other. Next, participants answered how close they were with their
roommates our key independent variable. Participants were asked, “How close were you to
your roommate, [name], at the time you requested the money?” on a 20-point Likert scale (1 =
[name] was like a complete stranger to me, 20 = [name] was like a close family member to me).
Lastly, participants answered whether they had the option to request the owed money via a
payment app (yes or no), how much money their roommate owed, how long they waited until
requesting their money back, and demographic variables such as age and gender. Following our
preregistration, only participants who correctly answered the attention check question about
whom they had to request back owed money and answered “yes” to having the option to request
owed money via a payment app were included in the final analysis.
1.3.2 Results
On average, the roommates owed $284.62, and participants waited 24.88 days before
requesting back the owed money. In-person requests were the most frequent (57.66%), followed
by texts (22.54%), digital payment apps (17.68%), emails (.88%), other (.87%), and phone calls
(.37%); see Appendix A for details).
Following our preregistration, we concentrated on the two request methods that most
differ in social richness based on the previously mentioned social richness pilot test (Appendix
A): digital payment apps versus in-person requests. To test our hypothesis, we created a
combination of dummy variables by comparing the choice of request made via digital payment
apps (coded as “1”) or in person (coded as “0”). With this combination of the dummy variables
13
(n = 605), we conducted a logistic regression predicting the choice of request method based on
how close they were with their roommate.
As hypothesized, regression analyses (see Table 1.2) revealed that the closeness with
one’s roommate is negatively associated with the choice of request made via digital apps (β = -
.22, SE = .10, z = -2.35, p = .019). This association persisted even after controlling for the
amount of money that was owed (β = -.22, SE = .10, z = -2.25, p = .024) and the number of days
waited until requesting the money back (β = -.22, SE = .10, z = -2.19, p = .029)
1
. These results
are consistent with H1: consumers adapt their approach to resolving peer indebtedness based on
the strength of their social connections with the requestee. Specifically, we found that, even
when participants had the opportunity to interact with their roommates frequently, weaker social
connections between participants and their roommates predicted a greater likelihood of
requesting money using digital apps and a lower likelihood of requesting in person.
TABLE 1.2: STUDY 1A RESULTS
Dependent variable: Choice of digital app (1) versus in-person (0) request
Independent variable
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
Closeness
-.23*
(.10)
-.22*
(.10)
-.22*
(.10)
-.22*
(.10)
Amount of Money Owed
-.88*
(.35)
-.46
(.36)
Number of days waited
-1.16***
(.34)
-1.02**
(.36)
NOTE. *p < .05, **p < .01, *** p < .001
1
Although non-preregistered, because text requests were common, comparisons between 1) digital payment app
versus text and 2) text versus in-person requests can be found in Appendix A.
14
1.4 Study 1B: Retrospective Study Across Contexts and
Connections
The goal of Study 1B was to replicate the patterns from Study 1A, but this time using a
broader set of contexts. As in Study 1A, we expected consumers who requested money from
weak versus strong social ties would prefer to use digital apps rather than ask in person.
1.4.1 Methods
We recruited 252 MTurk participants via CloudResearch between the ages of 18 and 35
(M
Age
= 28.10, 53.17% female). After excluding participants who failed our preregistered
exclusion criteria, a final sample of 192 participants remained. The preregistration for this study
is here: https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=gu8v7x.
Participants first answered two questions about whether they had ever been in a situation
where 1) a close friend and/or 2) a distant acquaintance owed them money, and they had to
request the money back from that person. Participants who answered “yes” to either question
were eligible for the study and typed the first name or initials of the close friend and/or distant
acquaintance from whom they had requested money back. Participants next answered open-
ended questions describing the situation (e.g., why they lent the money), how much money they
lent, and how long they waited (in days) before requesting the money back. After describing
each situation, participants answered our main dependent variable question how they requested
the owed funds from their close friend or distant acquaintance from among five options: 1) in-
person (face-to-face), 2) over a phone call, 3) via text message, 4) via a payment app such as
Venmo, Zelle, PayPal, or Facebook Messenger, or 5) other.
15
1.4.2 Results
Out of 192 participants, 178 (92.71%) reported that they had been in a situation where a
close friend owed them money, and they had to request it back, and 94 (48.96%) reported that
they had been in a situation where a distant acquaintance owed them money and they had to
request it back (see Appendix A for details).
Using an approach similar to Study 1, we created three combinations of dummy variables
to code the choice of request method, following our preregistration: 1) digital payment apps
versus in-person, 2) digital payment apps versus phone calls and texts, and 3) phone calls and
texts versus in-person. While comparison 1 (digital apps versus in-person) is our primary
interest, we also compared phone and text requests to explore how media that are moderate in
social richness (Appendix A) differ from the media on both ends of the social richness scale.
We conducted three separate chi-square tests comparing the choice of request method by
requestee relationship (strength of social tie) with each combination of the dummy variables.
Table 1.3 displays the raw counts and percentages of the request methods for each combination.
TABLE 1.3: STUDY 1B RESULTS
Requestee
Relationship
Request Method
Chi-square test
Comparison 1
Digital Payment
App
In-Person
Close Friend
18.87%
(20/106)
81.13%
(86/106)
χ
2
(1, N = 170) = 8.36, p =
.004
Distant
Acquaintance
39.06%
(25/64)
60.94%
(39/64)
Comparison 2
16
Requestee
Relationship
Request Method
Chi-square test
Digital Payment
App
Phone & Text
Close Friend
22.47%
(20/89)
77.53%
(69/89)
χ
2
(1, N = 140) = 10.48, p =
.001
Distant
Acquaintance
49.02%
(25/51)
50.98%
(26/51)
Comparison 3
Phone & Text
In-person
Close Friend
44.52%
(69/155)
55.48%
(86/155)
χ
2
(1, N = 220) = .38, p =
.54
Distant
Acquaintance
40%
(26/65)
60%
(39/65)
Our preregistered analyses showed a significant relationship between the request method
choice and the requestee's relationship when comparing digital payment apps versus in-person
requests. In support of H1, when looking only at participants who requested either via digital
apps or in person (comparison 1), participants requested money using digital apps more
frequently when requesting from distant acquaintances (39.06%) than when requesting from
close friends (18.87%; χ2(1, N = 170) = 8.36, p = .004). Correspondingly, more participants
requested money in person (81.13%) from close friends than from distant acquaintances
(60.94%). That is, participants were more likely to use digital apps and less likely to request in
person when requesting money from weak versus strong social ties.
Further, we found a significant relationship when comparing digital payment app requests
versus phone and text requests (comparison 2). When looking only at participants who requested
either via digital apps or phone/text, participants once again more frequently requested money
17
via digital payment apps from distant acquaintances (49.02%) than from close friends (22.47%;
χ2(1, N = 140) = 10.48, p = .001); correspondingly, more participants requested via phone or text
with close friends (77.53%) than distant acquaintances (50.98%). This pattern eases any
remaining concerns that patterns in Study 1A occurred because roommates who are socially
closer to each other have more opportunities to request in-person. In this comparison, we focus
only on phone-based options and still see that participants select socially rich approaches more
often when requesting from close social connections. For the final comparison (comparison 3),
no difference arose comparing phone and text versus in-person requests between close friends
(44.52%) and distant acquaintances (40%; χ2(1, N = 220) = .38, p = .54).
1.4.3 Discussion
Study 1A and 1B both show support for H1 consumers’ approach to resolving peer
indebtedness depends on the strength of their social ties with the requestee when we look at
reports of their actual requesting behavior. Consumers frequently request repayment using
socially rich methods (i.e., in-person requests) from their peers. However, with weaker social ties
such as distant acquaintances, consumers’ preference for less socially rich methods (e.g., digital
app requests) increases.
One advantage of the methods in Studies 1A and 1B is that we examined participants’
recall of real instances of their financial request behaviors. Moreover, the roommate setting in
Study 1A suggests that these patterns occur even in settings where consumers see the requestee
frequently. However, these studies do not demonstrate causality. Various factors such as
payment amount and loan circumstances may also vary across these relationship types,
potentially contributing to the differences in their choice of request method. Thus, in the
18
following studies, we use experimental designs to investigate and isolate whether the strength of
the social tie between the requester and requestee drives the choice of request method.
1.5 Study 2: Stimulus Sampling
Study 2 provides further support for H
1
by directly manipulating the strength of the social
connection between the requester and requestee and otherwise holding the situation constant. In
addition, we implemented stimulus sampling, systematically presenting nine different scenarios
across participants to verify the generalizability of the phenomena under study and reduce the
threat of idiosyncratic features within any individual scenario (Judd, Westfall, and Kenny 2012;
Wells and Windschitl 1999). We expect that, across scenarios, individuals requesting from a
distant acquaintance relative to a close friend would be more likely to request owed money via
digital apps and, accordingly, would be less likely to request in person.
1.5.1 Methods
We recruited 1505 Amazon MTurk participants via CloudResearch (M
Age
= 39.58,
49.97% female). Participants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions in a
2-cell (requestee: close friend vs. distant acquaintance) between-subjects design. Each participant
was then randomly assigned to view one of nine scenarios (amusement park, car troubles,
concert ticket, food, gas money, happy hour, housing, movie ticket, Uber ride). The amount of
money lent in each scenario varied from $10 to $100.
Participants imagined that either a close friend or a distant acquaintance named Charlie
had borrowed money (see Appendix B for materials). We describe the concert ticket scenario
below, but all scenarios follow a similar pattern. Participants in the concert ticket scenario
imagined that Charlie had forgotten their wallet and asked the participant to cover the cost of
19
their concert ticket. Participants were told that it had now been a couple of weeks since Charlie
had borrowed the money, and there were two ways to request the money back: either 1) request
via Venmo or 2) request in person. Venmo was described as a digital payment service that allows
users to request money through its phone app. Requesting in person was described as, “You can
request the money from Charlie face to face (assume you see Charlie twice a week and the
money will be paid through a digital payment method).” We specified these assumptions for the
in-person request to hold constant other factors that could affect the choice of request method,
such as how often the participant sees Charlie and the method by which they would receive
payment due to convenience. Participants then answered the question, “How would you ask for
the money Charlie, [one of your closest friends/one of your distant acquaintances], owed?”
Participants could respond either “Ask via Venmo” or “Ask in person.”
1.5.2 Results
As outlined in our preregistration (https://aspredicted.org/K9M_Q6D), only participants
who correctly answered the attention check questions such as who the requestee was and how
often they see the requestee (N = 1140) were included in the final analysis. We conducted a
logistic regression to regress participants’ choice of request method (1 = Ask via Venmo; 0 =
Ask in person) on the requestee (1 = distant acquaintance, 0 = close friend), including fixed
effects for scenarios. As predicted, participants in the distant acquaintance condition (45.49%;
237/521) were more likely to request owed money via a digital app than were participants in the
close friend condition (27.30%; 169/619; z = 6.45, p < .001); correspondingly, participants in the
close friend condition were more likely to request in person (72.70%; 450/619) than were
participants in the distant acquaintance condition (54.51%; 284/521). That is, when participants
requested owed money from a weak social tie (i.e., a distant acquaintance) compared to a strong
20
social tie (i.e., a close friend), they were more likely to request via a digital app and less likely to
ask in person. The pattern of results was also directionally consistent when looking at each
scenario separately (see Table 1.4).
TABLE 1.4: STUDY 2 RESULTS
Scenario
Close friend
Distant acquaintance
p-value
Amusement Park
31.75%
54.90%
p = .013
Bar happy hour
22.86%
42.11%
p = .02
Car troubles
18.42%
32.20%
p = .06
Concert ticket
27.50%
49.15%
p = .009
Food
25.35%
35.82%
p = .18
Gas money
33.96%
46.00%
p = .21
Housing
23.73%
50.00%
p = .003
Movie ticket
24.64%
47.69%
p = .005
Uber ride
38.46%
54.72%
p = .07
Collapsed
27.30%
45.49%
p < .001
1.5.3 Discussion
Study 2 provided additional evidence that, across multiple consumer contexts, individuals
have different preferred methods for requesting owed money based on the strength of social
connection with the requestee. When the requestee is an acquaintance relative to a close friend,
consumers are more likely to request via a digital app and less likely to request in person.
21
1.6 Study 3: Mediating Role of In-Person Request
Discomfort and Aversion to Appearing Impersonal
Our framework predicts that both the discomfort of requesting in person and the aversion
to appearing impersonal mediate the relationship between one’s social tie strength with the
requestee and their choice of request method. In Study 3, we expect to replicate the results of
Studies 1A-2: people will be more likely to request owed money via digital apps with a distant
acquaintance versus a close friend. Further, we predict that when requesting money from a
distant acquaintance (vs. a close friend), individuals would feel more discomfort requesting in
person, which in turn increases requests via digital apps (H
2
). We additionally predict that
individuals would be less averse to appearing impersonal when requesting money from a distant
acquaintance (vs. a close friend), further contributing to the preference for using digital apps with
distant social connections to request funds (H
3
).
1.6.1 Methods
We recruited 401 Amazon MTurk participants via CloudResearch (M
Age
= 39.91, 46.13%
female). Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions (requestee: close friend
vs. distant acquaintance) in a between-subjects design. Each participant was then randomly
assigned to view one of three scenarios (concert ticket, food, Uber ride). Following our
preregistration (https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=5un8ta), we planned to collapse analyses
across all scenarios, focusing our analysis and interpretation on the two different requestee
relationship conditions.
22
In Study 3, participants provided the first name of either a close friend or a distant
acquaintance. Participants imagined requesting owed money from that individual following one
of three scenarios (concert ticket, food, or Uber ride; see Appendix B) and were asked, “How
would you request the money that [name of the close friend/distant acquaintance] owed?” We
then measured our two proposed mechanisms: 1) the discomfort of requesting in person and 2)
the aversion to appearing impersonal. We asked participants in all conditions to rate their
discomfort of requesting the money in person using three items (adapted from Jiang, Hoegg, and
Dahl 2013): a) “How uncomfortable…, b) “How awkward…, and c) “How confrontational
would you feel when requesting money from [name of the requestee], your [close friend/distant
acquaintance] in person?” (1 = Not at all, 7 = Very much; α = .89). To measure the aversion to
appearing impersonal, we again used three items (adapted from Short, Williams, and Christie
1976): “How much of a problem would it be if [name of the requestee], your [close friend/distant
acquaintance] saw you as a) a cold person, b) impersonal, and c) insincere?” (1 = Not much of a
problem, 7 = Very much of a problem; α = .95). The order in which participants saw the two sets
of items was randomized.
1.6.2 Results
Replicating previous results, we found that participants were more likely to request owed
money via a digital app in the distant acquaintance condition (44.88%; 92/205) than in the close
friend condition (31.63%; 62/196; χ
2
(1, N = 401) = 7.43, p = .006). That is, when requesting
owed money from a weak (versus a strong) social tie, participants were more likely to request via
a digital app and, consequently, less likely to request in person. The pattern of results was also
consistent when looking at each scenario separately (see Appendix A).
23
As predicted, participants in the distant acquaintance condition (M = 4.14, SD = 1.74)
reported more discomfort requesting in person than those in the close friend condition (M = 3.43,
SD = 1.73; t(399) = 4.09, p < .001, d = .41, 95% CI [.21, .61]). Also as predicted, participants
were less averse to appearing impersonal in the distant acquaintance condition (M = 3.16, SD =
1.66) compared to the close friend condition (M = 4.57, SD = 1.74; t(399) = -8.30, p < .001, d = -
.83, 95% CI [-1.03, -.63]).
We tested whether the effect of the requestee relationship on the choice of request
method was mediated by the 1) discomfort of requesting in person and 2) aversion to appearing
impersonal (Hayes 2017, Model 4). Contrasting the close friend (0) and distant acquaintance (1)
conditions, we simultaneously tested the significance of both measured mediators by calculating
the standardized effects for 10,000 bootstrapped samples. We found a statistically significant
indirect effect of the discomfort of requesting in person (indirect effect = .24; 95% CI [.11, .41]),
and the indirect effect of the aversion to appearing impersonal was marginally significant
(indirect effect = .17; 95% CI [-.02, .38]).
2
As illustrated in Figure 1.2, including the two
mediators in the model, the direct effect of requestee relationship on the choice of request
method was no longer significant, indicating full mediation (direct effect = .20, p = .41, 95% CI
[-.27, .67]). In sum, the proposed mechanisms jointly and fully mediated the effect of requestee
relationship on the choice of request method. These results suggest that participants were more
uncomfortable requesting in person and less averse to appearing impersonal with distant
acquaintances versus close friends, thus increasing requests via digital apps for those weak social
ties.
2
When excluding participants who never used mobile payment apps, we found a significant indirect effect of
aversion to appearing impersonal (indirect effect = .26, 95% CI [.02, .52]).
24
FIGURE 1.2: STUDY 3 MEDIATION MODEL
NOTE.
p <.10, *p < .05, **p < .01, *** p < .001
1.6.3 Discussion
Study 3 provided evidence for the hypothesized mediators driving our effect. Consumers
feel more discomfort requesting money in person from an acquaintance versus a close friend,
which increases their likelihood to request through digital means instead. In addition, individuals
are less averse to appearing impersonal when requesting money from an acquaintance versus a
close friend, thus increasing their digital (vs. in-person) requests.
1.7 Study 4A: Moderation via Decreased Discomfort
Study 3 found initial evidence that the increased discomfort of requesting in person and
the decreased aversion to appearing impersonal when interacting with distant acquaintances
25
increase consumers’ preference for requesting money using digital apps. Studies 4A and 4B
sought additional evidence of our proposed mechanisms via moderation (Spencer, Zanna, and
Fong 2005).
In Study 4A, we directly manipulated the discomfort of requesting in person by
describing the requestee as someone who is approachable. If consumers feel discomfort when
requesting owed money from distant acquaintances in person, then assuring minimal discomfort
should influence their choice of approach. Accordingly, we expect a reduction in the preference
for digital requests when a distant acquaintance is described as a person who is easy to talk to.
However, given that discomfort is already low for close friends, we expect a more modest
decrease in digital requests when this description is used for a close friend.
1.7.1 Methods
Amazon MTurk participants (N = 1201; MAge = 37.16, 50.12% female) were recruited
via CloudResearch. Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions in a 2
(requestee: close friend vs. distant acquaintance) x 2 (discomfort: control vs. decreased)
between-subjects design. Each participant was then randomly assigned to view one of three
scenarios (concert ticket, food, Uber ride). In accordance with our preregistration
(https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=7jf7ep), only participants that answered the attention check
questions correctly (N = 1085) were included in the analysis.
As in Study 3, participants imagined either a close friend or a distant acquaintance named
Charlie had borrowed money for a concert ticket, food, or an Uber ride and that they had to
request the money that was owed. Half of the participants read that Charlie is very friendly and
easy to talk to for the decreased discomfort manipulation, while the other half (control) did not
26
read this description. Then participants chose how they would prefer to request money from
Charlie (either “using Venmo” or “Ask in person”).
1.7.2 Results
Following our preregistration, we collapsed across the three scenarios and conducted a
logistic regression of the choice of request method on dummy variables representing close friend
versus distant acquaintance, discomfort conditions, and the interaction between these terms. This
analysis yielded a significant main effect of requestee relationship (z = -5.34, p < .001) and a
main effect of the discomfort manipulation (z = -3.80, p < .001). Most important, we found a
marginally significant interaction between requestee relationship and discomfort (z = 1.75, p =
.081). Probing this interaction, we found that participants were more likely to request the money
via a digital app from a distant acquaintance (58.36%; 157/269) than from a close friend
(35.46%; 100/282; χ
2
(1, N = 551) = 29.02, p < .001) in the discomfort-control condition where
no additional information about the requestee was provided. In the discomfort-decreased
condition, however, when Charlie was described as “very friendly and easy to talk to,the
difference in digital app requests between a distant acquaintance (41.76%; 109/261) versus a
close friend (30.4%; 83/273; χ
2
(1, N = 534) = 7.48, p = .006) was still significant, but smaller
(see Figure 1.3). The pattern of results was also consistent when looking at each scenario
separately (see Appendix A).
FIGURE 1.3: STUDY 4A RESULTS
27
1.7.3 Discussion
If individuals prefer using digital apps to request money from acquaintances to avoid the
discomfort of fiscal confrontation, reducing discomfort should influence their preferred
approach. Results from Study 4A are in line with this conjecture informing participants that an
acquaintance (versus a close friend) is approachable resulted in a greater decrease in the choice
to request via a digital app and, consequently, increased the choice to request in person.
Therefore, we found causal evidence that individuals choose to request via digital apps with
acquaintances partly due to the discomfort they expect when requesting in person; this
discomfort looms larger with acquaintances than with close friends.
28
1.8 Study 4B: Moderation via Impersonal Discomfort
Study 4B examines our other proposed mechanism: the aversion to appearing impersonal.
We manipulated participants’ aversion to appearing impersonal by describing the requestee as
someone who values warm interactions, thus making it important not to appear impersonal when
requesting the money. We predict that consumers are typically highly averse to appearing
impersonal when requesting owed money from close friends but less so when requesting money
back from distant acquaintances. Therefore, emphasizing that participants should avoid
appearing impersonal should have less of an effect on participants requesting from close friends,
who already focus on this dimension, but a larger effect on participants requesting from distant
acquaintances.
1.8.1 Methods
We recruited 1402 Prolific Academic participants (M
Age
= 28.36, 44.01% female).
Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions in a 2 (requestee: close friend vs.
distant acquaintance) x 2 (impersonal: control vs. averse) between-subjects design. Each
participant was then randomly assigned to view one of three scenarios (concert ticket, food, Uber
ride). Per our preregistration (https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=a2nv23), only participants who
correctly answered the attention check questions (N = 1291) were included in the analysis.
As in Study 4A, participants imagined either a close friend or a distant acquaintance
named Charlie had borrowed money for a concert ticket, food, or an Uber ride and that they had
to request the money that was owed. Participants in the impersonal-averse conditions read that
Charlie is someone who values warm interactions, so it is critical in this situation for them to not
appear impersonal when requesting the money. Participants in the impersonal-control conditions
29
did not have this information. Then participants chose how they would prefer to request money
from Charlie (either “using PayPal” or “Ask in person”).
3
1.8.2 Results
We collapsed across the three scenarios, following our preregistration, and conducted a
logistic regression of the choice of request method on dummy variables representing close friend
versus distant acquaintance, impersonal conditions, and the interaction between these terms. This
analysis yielded a non-significant main effect of requestee relationship (z = -.83, p = .41) and a
main effect of the impersonal manipulation (z = 7.90, p < .001). Most important, we found a
significant interaction between requestee and the impersonal manipulation (z = -2.95, p = .003).
In the impersonal-control conditions, participants were more likely to request the money using a
digital app with distant acquaintances (41.74%; 134/321) than with close friends (18.60%;
64/344; χ
2
(1, N = 665) = 42.52, p < .001). However, when we highlighted an aversion to
appearing impersonal (impersonal-averse conditions), this difference decreased and was no
longer significant (distant acquaintance: 11.78%; 35/297; close friend: 9.73%; 32/329; χ
2
(1, N =
626) = .69, p = .41; see Figure 1.4). The pattern of results was consistent when looking at each
scenario separately (see Appendix A).
FIGURE 1.4: STUDY 4B RESULTS
3
We changed the digital app choice option from “Venmo” to PayPal” because the recruited sample contains
participants from the United Kingdom who most widely use PayPal.
30
1.8.3 Discussion
Study 4B provided further evidence for the role of consumers’ aversion to appearing
impersonal in their choice of repayment request method. When consumers request owed money
from a distant acquaintance (vs. a close friend), they are more likely to use digital apps because
they are less averse to appearing impersonal in these relationships. However, we find an
attenuation of this effect when the distant acquaintance is described as someone with whom the
consumer does not wish to appear impersonal.
1.9 Study 5: Requesting Money Back from a Vendor
In our final study, we sought to broaden our investigation in two ways. First, we wished
to test a logical extension of our conceptualization: as consumers’ relationship with a requestee
becomes increasingly distant, we predict that financial interactions will become less socially rich
as well. We therefore added a third experimental condition in which participants requested
31
money back from a store vendor. We also included a new choice option, the least socially rich
request method from our pilot test (i.e., a “standard bank app”), to extend the boundaries of our
stimuli on social richness. While Venmo and a standard bank app are similar in that they are both
primarily used for digital transactions, a standard bank app lacks some of the social
communication features of Venmo (e.g., text and emoji features), leading to less socially rich
evaluations, as verified by our second pilot study. We primarily expect to replicate the main
pattern consumers are more likely to request owed money from weak social connections
(distant acquaintances and store vendors) using less socially rich media (Venmo and a standard
bank app) than when requesting from strong social ties (close friends). Moreover, we predict that
consumers would be more likely to request owed money via a standard bank app, the least social
option, with the most distant relationship (a vendor) than when requesting from a close friend or
a distant acquaintance.
Second, we wanted to explore how the strength of social connection affects consumers’
decision to opt out of requesting altogether In practice, the potential discomfort and relationship
threat consumers anticipate when requesting owed money may deter them from asking (e.g.,
Jaroszewicz 2020). We wished to test whether the previously observed differences in
communication choice persist when consumers can opt out of requesting money back.
1.9.1 Methods
We recruited 652 Prolific Academic participants (MAge = 33.36, 51.84% female).
Participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions (close friend, distant
acquaintance, vendor) in a between-subjects experimental design. Following our preregistration
(https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=yd6rn7), only participants who correctly answered the
32
attention check question, such as how often they see the requestee (N = 631), were included in
the final analysis.
Participants in the close friend and distant acquaintance conditions imagined that either
their close friend or distant acquaintance named Charlie owed them money for breaking a set of
their dishes. Participants in the vendor condition imagined that they had bought a set of dishes
from a store vendor but discovered that the dishes were broken when they arrived home. The
vendor’s policy is that if any of the dishes are broken, consumers can request a refund in person
during their next visit or take a photo of the broken dishes and request the refund electronically
(see Appendix B for scenario details).
Participants then answered the question, “How would you request the money from [name
of close friend/name of distant acquaintance/the pottery vendor]?” Participants could respond
“Use Venmo,” “Ask in person,” “Use a standard bank app,” or “Do not request the money back.”
Venmo was described as a digital payment service that allows users to request money through its
phone app. Requesting in person was described as, “You will request the money from
[Charlie/the vendor] face-to-face. The standard bank app was described as, “another standard
bank app on your phone that allows users to request money through a phone app.”
1.9.2 Results
Table 1.5 displays the raw counts and percentages of the request methods for each
requestee relationship. A chi-square analysis revealed a significant relationship between
requestee and the choice of request method (χ
2
(6, N = 631) = 52.93, p < .001).
TABLE 1.5: STUDY 5 RESULTS
33
Requestee
Relationship
Request Method
Venmo
In-person
Bank app
Do not request the money
back
Close Friend
(n = 199)
18.59%
(37/199)
56.28%
(112/199)
5.03%
(10/199)
20.1%
(40/199)
Distant
Acquaintance
(n = 228)
30.26%
(69/228)
47.81%
(109/228)
7.89%
(18/228)
14.04%
(32/228)
Vendor
(n = 204)
35.29%
(72/204)
49.02%
(100/204)
14.22%
(29/204)
1.47%
(3/204)
Following our preregistration, we created three combinations of dummy variables to code
the choice of request method: 1) Venmo and bank app (combined as “digital apps”) versus in-
person, 2) bank app versus Venmo and in-person, and 3) Do not request the money back versus
Venmo, in-person, and bank app. The first combination of dummy variables (Venmo and bank
app versus in-person) tested our prediction that when participants choose to request owed money
from weaker ties, such as a distant acquaintance or a vendor (relative to a close friend), they are
more likely to request using less social rich methods such as digital apps. The second
combination of dummy variables (bank app versus Venmo and in-person) tested our prediction
that the least socially rich request method (bank app) will more likely be used with the least
social relationship (vendor). The last combination of dummy variables (do not request the money
back versus Venmo, in-person, and bank app) explores how the strength of social ties affects
consumers’ decision to forgo owed money.
As predicted, pairwise comparisons showed that participants in the distant acquaintance
condition (44.39%; 87/196) were more likely to request owed money via digital apps (Venmo
and bank app) than participants in the close friend condition (29.56%; 47/159; χ
2
(1, N = 355) =
8.21, p = .004). That is, replicating previous findings, when consumers chose to request owed
34
money from a distant acquaintance compared to a close friend, they were more likely to request
using digital apps and less likely to request in person. We found no difference in the choice to
request using the bank app between the distant acquaintance (9.18%; 18/196) and close friend
(6.29%; 10/159) conditions (χ
2
(1, N = 355) = 1.01, p = .31). Lastly, participants in the close
friend condition (20.1%; 40/199) were marginally significantly more likely to forgo requesting
owed money than those in the distant acquaintance condition (14.04%; 32/228; χ
2
(1, N = 427) =
2.79, p = .095).
Participants in the vendor condition (50.25%; 101/201) were also more likely to request
owed money using digital apps than participants in the close friend condition (29.56%; 47/159;
χ
2
(1, N = 360) = 15.70, p < .001). Furthermore, participants in the vendor condition (14.43%;
29/201) were more likely to request owed money via a standard bank app than were participants
in the close friend condition (6.29%; 10/159; χ
2
(1, N = 360) = 6.09, p = .014). Lastly,
participants in the close friend condition (20.1%; 40/199) were more likely to opt out of
requesting back owed money compared to the vendor condition (1.47%; 3/204; χ
2
(1, N = 303) =
36.68, p < .001).
We found no difference in the choice to use digital apps with a distant acquaintance
(44.39%; 87/196) and a vendor (44.39%; 87/196; χ
2
(1, N = 397) = 1.37, p = .24). Contrary to our
prediction, we also found no difference in the request made between the vendor (14.43%;
29/201) and distant acquaintance (9.18%; 18/196) conditions via a standard bank app (χ
2
(1, N =
397) = 2.61, p = .11), though patterns are directionally consistent with the prediction. Lastly,
participants in the distant acquaintance condition (14.04%; 32/228) were more likely to opt out
of requesting back owed money compared to the vendor condition (1.47%; 3/204; χ
2
(1, N = 303)
= 22.83, p < .001). The greater likelihood to forgo requesting money in the distant acquaintance
35
condition suggests that consumers do not view distant acquaintances as purely transactional
relationships but likely do view vendors this way.
1.9.3 Discussion
The current findings provided further support for our framework, showing that consumers
adaptively resolve indebtedness depending on the strength of their social connection. Replicating
previous studies, we found that consumers were more likely to request owed money using digital
apps with weak (versus strong) social ties. In addition, we found that more consumers requested
using the least socially rich means (i.e., a standard bank app) when interacting with weaker social
ties, such as a vendor, relative to stronger social ties, such as a close friend. This pattern indicates
that as relationships become more distant, the preferred means of financial interactions become
more distant as well. Moreover, we found evidence that consumers were more likely to forgo
requesting owed money with close social ties than with weaker social ties. This suggests that
opting out of settling financial scores with peers is relatively common, even if this means
absorbing the financial cost; however, consumers are unwilling to forgo this financial loss with
highly distant and potentially transaction-based social contacts, such as store vendors.
Importantly, we also show that our main findings persist even when consumers are offered the
option to opt out of requesting owed money back.
1.10 General Discussion
Requesting owed money from peers can be uncomfortable and potentially fraught with
relationship hazards. In the present research, we study how consumers navigate this challenge.
Across seven studies, we find that consumers tailor their approach to requesting owed money
based on the strength of their social connection with the requestee. In both retrospective recall
36
paradigms (Studies 1A and 1B) and a stimulus-sampling experiment (Study 2), we document that
consumers are more likely to request owed money using digital apps with weaker social
connections, controlling for how frequently they see the requestee. We further find via mediation
(Study 3) and moderation (Studies 4A and 4B) that the difference in request method based on
social closeness arises because when interacting with weak social ties, consumers anticipate
greater discomfort from requesting in person and are less averse to appearing impersonal,
resulting in greater preference for digital request options.
One could wonder whether the current findings align with work on communal versus
exchange relationship norms (Clark and Mills 1979, 1993, 2012). This influential body of
research broadly conceptualizes friends versus acquaintances as communal versus exchange
relationships, respectively (e.g., Ryu and Han 2009), due to the differing norms of reciprocity for
each relationship type. However, the theory of communal and exchange relationships does not
answer the key questions posed in the current research: 1) how do consumers resolve peer debt
with each relationship, and 2) why is there a difference in request preferences? The theory does
make clear predictions about whether consumers would try to resolve peer debt with friends
versus acquaintances. The norms surrounding reciprocity are stronger for exchange relationships
(acquaintances), so consumers should be more likely to request owed money from weaker social
ties. We find a version of this pattern in Study 5, where consumers are more likely to opt out of
requesting owed money from close friends relative to store vendors. However, we also find that
request rates look largely the same between close friends and distant acquaintances. We do not
rely on the communal versus exchange distinction in this work because it does not explain how
consumers behave differently with friends versus acquaintances in this domain, and it does not
clearly predict how consumers resolve indebtedness depending on relationship type. Instead, we
37
draw from research on social tie strength (Granovetter 1973) and social richness (Daft and
Lengel 1986; Short, Williams, and Christie, 1976) to generate predictions ultimately supported
by our findings.
If anything, communal and exchange relationship theory might predict the opposite of
our proposition regarding anticipated discomfort if weak social ties are conceptualized as
exchange relationships. Under their theorization, exchange relationships prioritize equity and the
amount of benefit that is exchanged. Therefore, consumers could anticipate less discomfort
requesting back owed money in person from weak (vs. strong) social ties, given the expectations
of receiving back what is owed. However, our results suggest this is not the case.
Lastly, communal versus exchange relationship theory might predict that consumers
actually have lower concerns about appearing impersonal with strong ties because close
relationship members are more willing to forgive and accept each other’s behaviors. However,
while it might be true that individuals in close relationships are more forgiving of each other’s
behavior, our findings suggest that concern for the greater value of these relationships (e.g.,
Garcia-Rada et al. 2021) is a stronger driver of choices in this domain.
Interpersonal indebtedness evokes substantial unease. The current research shows that the
strength of consumers’ social connections affects how they attempt to resolve such indebtedness.
To fight the fiscal awkwardness of requesting money in person, consumers rely on media low in
social richness to communicate with weak social connections about owed money. At the same
time, consumers use more sociable means to communicate about money issues with strong social
connections to avoid appearing impersonal. Thus, consumers selectively maneuver to resolve
indebtedness based on the needs of the social relationship at hand.
38
Chapter 2: When Bigger Is Not Always
Better: Periodic Donations Enhance
Perceived Corporate Social Responsibility
and Company Engagement
2.1 Introduction
Corporate social responsibility (CSR), a company’s status and actions regarding its
perceived societal obligations (Brown and Dacin 1997; Sen and Bhattacharya 2001), has gained
significant attention from marketers and consumers over the past few decades. Partaking in
socially responsible activities not only benefits the firm’s image through charitable credit but
also the societal issue they support. With the idea that firms bring shared value to society and the
company, CSR has become an essential marketing strategy for companies (Kramer and Porter
2011; Porter and Kramer 2006).
However, the critical challenge firms face when engaging in socially responsible
activities is getting the consumers to believe the firm’s authentic prosocial motives and
charitable commitment that go beyond financial obligations. Consumers doubt the authenticity of
charitable activities, seeing them as self-interested marketing strategies driven by profit. As a
result, they look for evidence of genuine prosocial motivation (Barasch et al. 2014; Newman and
Cain 2014). Despite the shared values CSR brings, skepticism towards these actions can
diminish or even harm the charitable reputation of companies (Berman et al. 2015; Yoon,
Gurhan-Canli, and Schwarz 2006). Thus, marketers are highly motivated to share their socially
responsible activities in ways that show their commitment to the prosocial cause.
39
The current research investigates how firms can mitigate consumers’ doubt of prosocial
commitment when advertising their CSR activities. Specifically, we propose that firms can
leverage a temporal framing strategy for their donations. Counter to the intuition that a larger
aggregate amount (e.g., donate $120,000 this year) is perceived to be more charitable, we show
that a smaller periodic amount (e.g., donate $10,000 each month this year) increases positive
perceptions of the company and engagement with the firm. We further show that the
effectiveness of periodic framing is driven by perceived commitment breaking a donation into
several instances helps to increase the goodwill consumers attribute to CSR because repeated
donations reinforce that a company is authentically committed to a cause. Thus, when companies
frame their donations periodically, consumers believe firms are more committed to the prosocial
cause, increasing positive perceptions and engagement.
Our findings make contributions to several bodies of literature. First, we contribute to the
literature on charitable credit and commitment. Previous research has largely emphasized the
importance intentions, motives, and commitment have on charitable credit (Berman et al. 2015;
Lin-Healy and Small 2013; Newman and Cain 2014; Small and Cryder 2016). The more genuine
and committed prosocial actions are, the greater the charitable credit they receive. Moreover,
prior findings have shown that consumers interpret repeated behaviors as more diagnostic of
others’ characteristics and intentions (D’Souza and Rao 1995; Skowronski and Carlston 1987;
Valsesia and Diehl 2022). Therefore, we extend this literature by showing how periodic framing
of donations a repeated action can increase perceptions of charitable commitment.
Second, we document an important theoretical moderator of temporal framing on
charitable credit. In contrast to the current findings, recent work showed that periodic donations
could actually harm the perceptions of the donors (Basu 2021). Specifically, the author found
40
that donors who made periodic (vs. aggregate) donations were perceived to be less morally
praiseworthy because the smaller donations under periodic framing were perceived to make less
of a sacrifice. Reconciling Basu (2021) and our work, we extend the current literature by
showing that total donation size matters when temporally framing donations. Specifically, we
document that when the total donation size is relatively small (e.g., $120 this year), periodic
framing of donations (e.g., $10 a month) decreases charitable credit, consistent with Basu (2021).
However, in larger amounts (e.g., $120,000 this year), we find a reversal of the effect such that
periodic donations (e.g., $10,000 a month) increase charitable credit because of the greater
perceived commitment towards the prosocial cause.
Lastly, we contribute to the work on temporal framing by showing that the strategy's
effectiveness goes beyond consumer-centric domains. Past findings examined how periodic
pricing can impact consumers' donation and purchase intentions through the role of perceived
costs and benefits of the contracts (e.g., Gourville 1998; Atlas and Bartels 2018). However, prior
work has primarily focused on how temporal framing is utilized as a pricing strategy to induce
consumption (e.g., subscription, donation intentions). The current work takes a novel perspective
by focusing on the effectiveness of temporal framing on firm-centric domains, specifically CSR.
In the following sections, we review the literature on corporate social responsibility and
charitable commitment. Then, drawing on the work on temporal framing, we describe the impact
of periodic framing on perceived commitment, charitable credit, and firm engagement.
41
2.2 Conceptual Background
2.2.1 Corporate Social Responsibility: A Double-Edged Sword
Corporate social responsibility creates shared value: CSR activities yield positive benefits
for society and enhance a firm’s image and reputation (Kramer and Porter 2011; Porter and
Kramer 2006). Engaging in CSR can generate positive attitudes towards firms (Brown and Dacin
1997; Sen, Bhattacharya, and Korschun 2006), brands (Klein and Dawar 2004), retail stores
(Lichtenstein, Drumwright, and Braig 2004), and company identification (Sen and Bhattacharya
2001), which impacts a firm’s revenue growth (Lev, Petrovits, and Radhakrishnan 2010; Mohr
and Webb 2005). These multi-faceted effects of CSR have led firms to adopt CSR as a core
marketing strategy.
Although consumers generally favor CSR activities, evidence suggests that CSR is
ineffective and can lead to consumer backlash if done improperly. When a company's brand
image contradicts its CSR information, disfluency between the two concepts arises, leading to a
negative evaluation of the company (Torelli, Monga, and Kaikati 2012). Similarly, CSR
initiatives backfire if brand concepts are at odds with a CSR campaign's message (Yoon,
Gurhan-Canli, and Schwarz 2006). Hence, CSR is more complex than donating to a cause. A
company should maximize the goodwill associated with CSR by understanding the factors that
lead good deeds to favorable judgments.
Growing work suggests that a critical factor that sways consumer favorability of a firm’s
CSR activities is the perception of a company’s motive. In charitable giving contexts, consumers
look for signals of authentic prosocial motivation (Barasch et al. 2014). When a company’s
financial motives are salient, CSR can appear at odds with the altruism it promotes, a domain in
42
which people believe empathy should be the primary driver of behavior (Batson et al. 1991). So,
firms elicit selfish cognitions if consumers perceive CSR as driven primarily by self-centered
and egoistic motives (Ellen, Webb, and Mohr 2006). Such findings are consistent with work in
the social judgment literature, where actors sharing good deeds can create favorable inferences of
generosity while simultaneously generating negative attributions of selfishness (Berman et al.
2015). Newman and Cain (2014) found that doing nothing at all was sometimes better than
charitable actions that generated negative attributions of selfishness, likely due to consumers
questioning the selfish versus selfless nature of good deeds (Critcher and Dunning 2014). Pure
altruism involves sacrifice (Lin-Healy and Small 2014), and any donation must be weighed
against the self-serving behavior of a brand. Given this tension between charitable giving and
ulterior motives, CSR may fail to provide evidence that their behaviors are not self-serving. Said
another way, without indicators of authentic prosocial motivation, companies that announce
charitable contributions might actually risk consumer backlash. So, firms face the challenge of
conveying authentic commitment when sharing their CSR activities.
2.2.2 Commitment and Charitable Credit
People generally attribute another’s behaviors to internal motivations if those behaviors
are consistent over time (Kelley 1973). Consistent behavior is viewed positively (Suh 2002), as a
signal of authenticity (English and Chen 2011; Kraus, Chen, and Keltner 2011), and as more
diagnostic of one’s characteristics and intentions (Skowronski and Carlston 1987; Valsesia and
Diehl 2022). Moreover, people believe that those who make small commitments are likely to
follow through with larger commitments that are consistent with these values (Cialdini,
Cacioppo, Bassett, and Miller 1978). These findings suggest that consistency within a firm’s
CSR activities may convey authentic commitment as consumers perceive a firm’s frequent
43
charitable acts as a stronger cue of commitment. So, we propose that making such authentic
commitment (dedication to a cause) salient through repetition should increase the effectiveness
of CSR by generating goodwill toward the company. This conjecture is consistent with findings
in the attribution theory literature, which suggest that increases in perceived company
commitment should lead consumers to react more favorably to a company’s actions (Cui, Trent,
Sullivan, and Matiru 2003; Dean 2003; Forehand and Grier 2003; Sparkman and Locander
1980).
An open question remains: How can firms highlight the repetition of charitable actions
that signal commitment when communicating their CSR activities? An ideal solution would be to
engage in CSR activities more frequently to show commitment. The more often firms act
socially responsible, the more committed they are perceived to be in support of the prosocial
cause. However, a practical limitation to this solution is that resources can be limited. Thus,
firms must resolve the issue of highlighting commitment through frequency without increasing
the donation size. We suggest that firms can leverage a temporal framing strategy for their
donations. Specifically, we propose that firms framing their donations as a series of periodic
donations can highlight the repetition of charitable actions, increasing charitable perceptions and
firm engagement.
2.2.3 Periodic Donations on Commitment and Charitable Perceptions
The literature on temporal framing and periodic pricing offers a strategy whereby
corporate donations can leverage perceived commitment. Just as higher payment frequencies
lead consumers to believe they have more resources (De la Rosa and Tully 2022), a higher
frequency of donations may increase the degree to which consumers believe a company is
44
dedicating to a cause. Related evidence supports that repeated behaviors can impact consumer
attributions and engagement. For example, consumers read others’ characteristics and intentions
from repeated behaviors (Gershon and Smith 2020; Skowronski and Carlston 1987; Valsesia and
Diehl 2022) and make inferences about a company’s intentions when companies repeat
advertisements (D’Souza & Rao 1995). So, in the case of repeated donations, consumers would
infer that a firm’s series of periodic donations signal a greater commitment to charitable causes
than a single aggregate donation.
Moreover, single aggregate donations need help signaling commitment, despite their
larger numeric value. First, consumers are scope insensitive (Frederick and Fischhoff 1998;
Kahneman and Knetsch 1990). Although a stronger signal of charitable intent can be
synonymous with increasing the donation size for many companies, consumers’ valuations of the
information are not multiplicative relative to the magnitude. So, larger amounts do not
necessarily mean they will receive proportional charitable credit. Second, firms highlighting their
largest donations can create a “goodwill ceiling.” This means that in the absence of authentic
prosocial motivation cues (Barasch et al. 2014), the scope of a company’s perceived altruism is
capped at a donation amount (Frederick and Fischhoff 1998; Kahneman and Knetsch 1990).
Lastly, announcing a single large donation can backfire if consumers believe the donor has an
ulterior motive (such as rehabilitating a company’s negative reputation) or does not do enough to
address a cause. For example, people often believe companies donate to capitalize on the
publicity of a current event and are cynical about a company’s likelihood of continuing to help
the cause. Sometimes consumers are unaware of a company’s prior charitable commitments, or a
company is wading into a new donation context. In these instances, companies risk attracting
negative perceptions if consumers perceive them as opportunistic.
45
As such, consumers may use temporal cues to infer consistency and commitment. Indeed,
consumers perceive a company as more authentically committed to a cause when it enhances the
perceived consistency and commitment of its CSR activities. Thus, we hypothesize that when a
company presents its donations as a series of periodic donations (e.g., $10,000 each month)
instead of a single aggregate donation (e.g., $120,000 this year), consumers will likely recognize
the greater authentic commitment, which in turn increases charitable perceptions and firm
engagement.
2.2.4 Total Donation Size Matters
However, it is unclear whether the benefits of a periodic donation strategy always
outweigh the magnitude and weight of a single aggregated donation. Because periodic donations
sacrifice magnitude for frequency, consumers may view each firm’s periodic donation as too
small, leading to inferences that the firm is not doing enough. For example, consumers believe
disaggregated payments are smaller (compared to aggregated payments; Gourville 1998). Due to
conversational norms, consumers see changes expressed in smaller units of measurement as
smaller than an equivalent change expressed in larger units (Zhang and Schwarz 2012). If people
experience smaller-unit quantities as smaller overall, periodic donations may lead consumers to
judge companies as less authentically committed to a cause.
Moreover, donation size must be sufficiently large because research suggests that
consumers vary in their focus on unit size and quantity when making judgments. Monga and
Bagchi (2012) demonstrated evidence of a unitosity effect, whereby consumers evaluated
changes based on the relative size of the units. Whereas past research supports that people rely
on the numerosity heuristic and infer a greater overall size when there are greater units (Pelham,
46
Sumarta, and Myaskovsky 1994), Monga and Bagchi (2012) demonstrated that the reverse
occurs when the unit type is salient. That is, when the type of unit is salient, people rely on the
size of the unit itself (as opposed to the number of units) when inferring the scale of change (i.e.,
small unitslike inchesequate to small changes and large unitslike feetequate to large
changes).
In the context of donations, the unit is likely salient because it provides consumers with a
sense of the scale of goodwill to which a company is committing. Said another way, if a donation
unit is small, consumers are likely to react negatively to the perception that a company is
committing so little to help people, regardless of the number of instances. In cases where the unit
is less salient, such as the numerosity heuristic, people rely on the number of units to inform their
judgments. For example, people believe delays framed in small (vs. large) units will be longer
when they focus on the number of units displayed in the delay (Pelham, Sumarta, and
Myaskovsky 1994). When consumers assessed a warranty and focused on the number of units
(rather than the type of unit), they saw warranties as better when these warranties were broken
into smaller (vs. larger) units (Pandelaere, Briers, and Lembregts 2011).
In fact, one paper consistent with this notion found that people perceive donors who
disaggregate their contributions (e.g., donating $10 a month) as less moral than those who make
aggregate contributions (e.g., donating $120 a year; Basu 2021). Although this finding
challenges the merit of periodic donations as a useful strategy for improving perceived company
commitment, Basu (2021) focused on charitable contributions that were relatively small
compared to those typically made by companies. Therefore, we hypothesize that firms donating
an earnest amount stand to benefit from the added temporal cues of periodic donations because
each donation instance reinforces a company’s dedication to a cause. However, in contexts
47
where the donation quantity per instance is small, consumers may focus on the small size of the
quantity rather than the benefits that come from each donation instance, harming charitable
perceptions.
2.2.5 Overview of Studies
We test our hypotheses across seven preregistered studies. Across our studies, we
examine how participants perceive and engage with periodic donations. We find that when
companies frame their donations in periodic (vs. aggregate) terms, customers have more positive
perceptions of the companies and are more likely to show interest and engage with them. We
examine the robustness of our effect across donation type, donation amount, messaging,
charitable cause, and company.
Studies 1A and 1B test the hypotheses via two preregistered field experiments. In Study
2, we use stimulus sampling to verify the effect's generalizability across various donation causes
and donation amounts. Study 3 tests the proposed mechanism of perceived commitment and
simultaneously rules out alternative mechanisms of perceived costs and benefits. Study 4
replicates the mechanism findings with a stronger three-item measure of commitment. Study 4,
also explores the downstream consequences of commitment on social media engagement,
controlling for consumer inferences. Study 5 examines the limits of the effect when another
temporal donation marker (past commitment) is introduced. Finally, we test an important
boundary condition of the effect extremely small donations (Study 6). We report all conditions,
data exclusions (if any), and measures for each study (Simmons, Nelson, and Simonsohn 2012).
All studies are preregistered, and the study materials, including complete stimuli, measures, data,
48
and code can be found here:
https://researchbox.org/809&PEER_REVIEW_passcode=TQTQHA.
2.3 Study 1A: Field Experiment (Salt and Smoke)
In Study 1A, we partnered with a company called Salt and Smoke and conducted a field
experiment. Salt and Smoke is a restaurant in St. Louis, Missouri, that often donates to local
communities. We launched a marketing campaign that either promoted Salt and Smoke’s
donation pledge in a periodic or aggregate format and tested which campaign would elicit more
clicks to the company’s website. We predict that a periodic (vs. aggregate) framing of donations
will produce greater interest in the company.
2.3.1 Methods
Salt and Smoke had 75,514 customer subscribers to their email newsletter. Customers
were randomly assigned to one of two conditions in a 2-cell (donation framing: aggregate vs.
periodic) between-subjects design. In the periodic condition, participants saw a marketing
campaign that framed Salt and Smoke’s donation pledge in periodic terms, while participants in
the aggregate condition read the donation pledge framed in aggregate terms (see Figure 2.1).
Importantly, the total dollar amount was held constant across both conditions. Customers who
were interested in learning more about Salt and Smoke had the option to click a link that
redirected customers to the Salt and Smoke website. The number of customers that clicked on the
Salt and Smoke website was the outcome measure of interest.
49
2.3.2 Results
Following our preregistration (https://aspredicted.org/Y91_Z38), we examined the click-
through rate by donation framing. In support of our hypothesis, customers who have received the
periodic framing of donation pledges (.69%; 278/40,272) were more likely to click on the Salt
and Smoke website than customers who have received the aggregate framing of donations (.35%;
124/35,242); χ
2
(1) = 40.66, p < .001.
FIGURE 2.1: STUDY 1 STIMULI
50
2.4 Study 1B: Field Experiment (GiftAMeal)
In Study 1B, we aim to replicate and extend our findings by showing that the effect
persists not only in monetary donations but also in goods donations. We partnered with a
company called GiftAMeal and conducted a field experiment. GiftAMeal is a company that
donates meals to food banks each time customers take photos of their meal and shares them on
social media. We launched two Facebook advertisements that described GiftAMeal’s previous
donations in a periodic or aggregate format and tested which advertisement would elicit the most
link clicks to the company’s website. Again, we hypothesize that a periodic (vs. aggregate)
framing of donations will produce greater interest in the company.
2.4.1 Methods
Using Facebook’s split testing platform, we experimentally tested the effectiveness of
two advertisements (Orazi and Johnston 2020). We further specified that Facebook targets
individuals 18 years or older who speak English and live in the cities/states where GiftAMeal has
a presence. Following our preregistration (https://aspredicted.org/1Y2_WJL), the sample size
was determined by a total budget limit of $600 over one month. This budget resulted in a total
reach of 67,576 Facebook users.
We had three different ad designs (see Appendix D for materials). For each ad design,
two advertisements were launched: periodic and aggregate framing (see Figure 2.2). In the
periodic condition, participants saw ad designs that framed how many meals GiftAMeal donated
last year in periodic terms. Specifically, the ads read, “GiftAMeal donated 25,000 meals every
month to local communities last year.” In the aggregate condition, the donations were framed in
aggregate terms such that the ads read, “GiftAMeal donated 300,000 meals to local communities
51
last year.” All participants had the option to click on the advertisement, which would redirect
them to the GiftAMeal website. The number of participants that clicked on the GiftAMeal ad
was the outcome measure of interest.
2.4.2 Results
As preregistered, we collapsed across the three different types of designs in each
condition and examined the number of unique link clicks (i.e., the total number of users who
clicked on the advertisement) out of the total reach (i.e., the number of unique users exposed to
the advertisement). As predicted, Facebook users were more likely to click on the ads that
showed the donations in periodic terms (1.48%; 489/32,968) than the ads that showed the
donations in aggregate terms (1.22%; 421/34,608); χ
2
(1) = 9.04, p = .003 (see Table 2.1).
FIGURE 2.2: STUDY 1B STIMULI
TABLE 2.1: STUDY 1B RESULTS
52
Advertisement
Periodic Condition
Aggregate Condition
p-value
Smiling kid
1.36%
(318/23,328)
1.16%
(292/25,136)
.047
Join us
1.52%
(101/6,646)
1.15%
(84/7,332)
.053
Download app
1.19%
(75/6,310)
1.00%
(45/4,517)
.35
Collapsed
1.48%
(489/32,968)
1.22%
(421/34,608)
.003
2.4.3 Discussion
The results from Studies 1A and 1B support the hypothesis that periodic framing of donations
elicits greater customer engagement than aggregate donations. Moreover, it is interesting to note
that the effect holds for both monetary donations (Study 1A) and goods donations (Study 1B).
Next, we test how framing affects company perceptions across various donation scenarios and
donation amounts.
2.5 Study 2: Stimulus Sampling
Study 2 implements stimulus sampling by presenting nine different scenarios across
participants to verify the generalizability of the effect (Judd, Westfall, and Kenny 2012; Wells
and Windschitl 1999). We expect that, across scenarios, periodic framing of donations would
increase positive perceptions of the firm than aggregate donations.
53
2.5.1 Methods
We recruited 1,797 Prolific Academic participants (M
Age
= 36.17, 47.86% female).
Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions in a 2-cell (donation framing:
aggregate vs. periodic) between-subjects design. Each participant was then randomly assigned to
view one of nine scenarios (education, environmental, gender equality, health 1, health 2,
hunger, poverty, refugee, social activism; see Appendix D). The donation amount and donation
cause were different across the nine scenarios. Moreover, it is important to note that in all
scenarios, the total donation amount is smaller in the periodic (vs. aggregate) condition. In
accordance with our preregistration (https://aspredicted.org/5CS_9CG), only participants that
answered the attention check questions correctly (N = 1,665) were included in the analysis.
Participants read that a fictional company pledged to donate to a certain prosocial cause.
We describe the education scenario below, but all scenarios follow a similar pattern. Participants
in the education scenario read that a company named ANY Corporation recently pledged to
donate a portion of their earnings to support children’s education and public schools. The
donation pledge read,
“ANY Corporation recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant
amount of good. We pledge to donate a [aggregate condition: one-time donation of $1.2 million
in 2023/periodic condition: $90,000 every month between January 2023 and December 2023] to
non-profit organizations that support children’s education and public schools.
Participants then rated the key dependent variable of company perception using three
items: 1) “How much do you admire [company name]?”, 2) “How favorably do you view
[company name]?”, and 3) “How positively do you view [company name]?” (1 = Not at all, 7 =
54
Very much). The three items were averaged to create a “company perception” composite (α =
.95).
2.5.2 Results
Following our preregistered analysis, we regressed company perception on the framing of
donation pledges, including a fixed effect of scenarios. As predicted, participants in the periodic
condition (M = 5.47, SD = 1.37) had more positive perceptions of the company than the
aggregate condition (M = 5.18, SD = 1.27), β = .24, t(1655) = 4.87, p < .001. The pattern of
results is mostly consistent when looking at each scenario separately (see Table 2.2).
TABLE 2.2: STUDY 2 RESULTS
Scenario
Periodic Condition
Aggregate
Condition
p-value
Education cause
5.61
4.70
< .001
Environmental cause
5.63
5.40
.20
Gender equality cause
4.98
5.01
.88
Health 1 cause
5.82
5.44
.048
Health 2 cause
5.68
5.27
.018
Hunger cause
5.47
5.34
.47
Poverty cause
5.54
5.28
.18
Refugee cause
5.64
5.07
.006
Social activism cause
5.06
5.06
.99
55
2.5.3 Discussion
Across multiple donation contexts and amounts, we find that donations framed in
periodic (vs. aggregate) terms increase charitable perceptions of the company. Moreover, we find
the effect persists even when the total amount of donation is smaller in the periodic condition
than the aggregate condition. Next, we test our proposed mechanism of perceived company
commitment while controlling for participants’ inferences about the donation amount.
2.6 Study 3: Mechanisms
Study 3 examines perceived commitment as a mechanism while ruling out alternative
mechanisms of perceived costs and benefits suggested by previous work on periodic pricing
(Atlas and Bartels 2018; Gourville 1998). Past work has documented that periodic pricing (e.g.,
$1/day) leads to greater purchase intentions than aggregate pricing (e.g., $365 a year) because of
either the lesser perceived costs (Gourville 1998) or greater perceived benefits of the purchase
(Atlas and Bartels 2018). Therefore, it is possible that the favorability judgment of periodic
donations is driven by these reasons. If true, we should expect to see a significant indirect(s) of
perceived costs, benefit, or both. However, if our proposed mechanism is true, we should only
see a significant indirect effect of perceived commitment.
2.6.1 Methods
We recruited 600 Prolific Academic participants (M
Age
= 34.53, 47.50% female). As in
Study 2, participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions in a 2-cell (donation
framing: aggregate vs. periodic) between-subjects design. Each participant was then randomly
assigned to view one of three scenarios (environmental, hunger, refugee donations; see Appendix
56
D). In accordance with our preregistration (https://aspredicted.org/MCR_ZZR), only participants
that answered the attention check questions correctly (N = 581) were included in the analysis.
The design mimics Study 2. Participants rated company perception (α = .94) after reading
a company’s donation pledge that was either framed periodically or in aggregate. We then
measured perceived commitment with the following item: “How committed is [company name]
to [donation cause]?” (1 = Not at all committed, 7 = Very committed). Moreover, we measured
the perceived costs (α = .82) and benefits (α = .64) of the donation using two sets of three seven-
point Likert questions (adapted from Atlas and Bartels 2018; see Appendix D). The order in
which participants saw the items of perceived commitment, costs, and benefits was randomized.
2.6.2 Results
Following our preregistered analysis, we regressed company perception on the framing of
donation pledges, including a fixed effect of scenarios. Replicating previous studies, participants
in the periodic condition (M = 5.27, SD = 1.22) had greater positive perceptions of the company
than the aggregate condition (M = 4.88, SD = 1.29), β = .29, t(577) = 3.58, p < .001. The pattern
of results is directionally consistent when looking at each scenario separately (see Appendix C).
Similarly, we found that participants in the periodic condition (M = 5.00, SD = 1.46) had
greater positive perceptions of the company than the aggregate condition (M = 4.49, SD = 1.52),
β = .32, t(577) = 3.99, p < .001. The pattern of results is directionally consistent when looking at
each scenario separately (see Appendix C).
Unlike previous work on periodic pricing (Atlas and Bartels 2018; Gourville 1998), we
found no significant differences of perceived costs, β = .09, t(577) = 1.16, p = .25, or benefits, β
57
= .07, t(577) = .82, p = .41, between participants in the periodic and aggregate conditions (see
Appendix C).
As shown in Figure 2.3, we conducted a parallel mediation model (Hayes 2017, Model 4)
with our donation framing as the independent variable (aggregate condition = 0, periodic
condition = 1); perceived commitment, costs, and benefits as parallel mediators; and company
perception as the dependent variable. As predicted, perceived commitment significantly
mediated the relationship between donation framing (indirect effect = .27; 95% CI [.14, .41]),
while perceived costs (indirect effect = .01; 95% CI [-.01, .03]) and benefits (indirect effect =
.02; 95% CI [-.02, .06] did not.
FIGURE 2.3: STUDY 3 MEDIATION MODEL
NOTE.
*p < .05, **p < .01, *** p < .001
58
2.6.3 Discussion
Study 3 provides evidence for the hypothesized mechanism of perceived commitment
while ruling out alternative explanations of perceived costs and benefits. When donations are
framed in periodic (vs. aggregate) terms, consumers believe the company is more committed to
the prosocial cause, increasing positive perceptions of the company. Study 4 seeks additional
evidence of our proposed mechanism.
2.7 Study 4: Perceived Commitment
Study 4 tests our mechanism of perceived commitment using another measure of
commitment, while controlling for consumers’ inferences about the total donation. More
specifically, we test whether periodic donations signal greater charitable commitment and elicit
greater consumer engagement than aggregate donations, even when an observer knows the total
donation. We hypothesize that consumers who see a periodic donation (whether in the presence
or absence of the total donation amount) will perceive the company as more committed to a
charitable cause than those who see an aggregate donation, increasing consumer engagement
with the company that donates periodically.
2.7.1 Methods
We recruited 748 Amazon Mechanical Turk participants (M
Age
= 39.90, 61.10% female).
Participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions in a 3-cell (donation framing:
aggregate vs. periodic vs. periodic-sum) between-subjects design. Following our preregistration
(https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=J6C_XXN), we excluded participants who either failed an
attention check or a manipulation check, leaving a total sample of 681 in the analysis.
59
Participants were asked to read a social media post. The post was a tweet by a materials
company touting its recent donation to child hunger charities. The tweet described the company’s
donation and a link to an article with more information (see Appendix D for materials).
Participants in the periodic condition saw the following text: “Today we are committing to
addressing child hunger by donating $10K a month for a year to multiple local charities. Click
the link to find out more.” Participants in the aggregate condition saw the following text: “Today
we are committing to child hunger by donating $120K to multiple local charities. Click the link
to find out more.” We also introduced a periodic-sum condition, where participants saw a tweet
with the following text, “Today we are committing to addressing child hunger by donating $10K
a month for a year (totaling $120K) to multiple local charities. Click the link to find out more.”
After seeing the tweet, all participants were instructed to briefly summarize the social
media post. We then measured our proposed mechanism of perceived commitment. Participants
rated the company’s commitment, dedication, and devotion by responding to the following
prompt: “Regarding child hunger efforts, how much do this company's actions show genuine
________?” (1 = Not at all, 7 = Extremely). These items showed strong reliability (α = .97).
Following our preregistration, we averaged the items to create a perceived commitment
composite. Then, participants responded to a series of items to assess their degree of engagement
with the company. More specifically, we asked, “If Barksdale Materials were to include the
following website links in their tweet, how likely would you be to click each:”, 1) “A link to
their website”, 2) “A link to a charity”, and 3) “The story linked in the tweet” (0 = Not at all, 10
= Definitely). These items showed high reliability (α = .90), so we created an engagement
composite using the average of these items. We additionally measured two exploratory
dependent variables (see Appendix C for results).
60
2.7.2 Results
We examined the effects of our periodic manipulations on the commitment composite
and found a significant difference between the conditions, F(2, 678) = 7.63, p < .001, η
p
2
= .022.
We then conducted a series of t-tests to examine differences across conditions. We first
examined whether the effect replicated. As predicted, participants believed a company was more
committed to a cause when announcing a periodic donation (M = 5.44, SD = 1.52) than when
announcing an aggregated donation (M = 5.01, SD = 1.51), t(464) = 3.05, p = .002, d = .28.
We next examined whether the periodic donation effect held when including the total
donation amount. As predicted, participants in the periodic-sum condition thought a company
was more committed to a cause (M = 5.50, SD = 1.37) than those in the aggregate condition,
t(453) = 3.60, p < .001, d = .34. There were no differences between the two periodic conditions,
t(439) = .44, p = .66.
We examined the effects of our periodic donation manipulations on our engagement
composite. Participants differed across conditions in the degree to which they were likely to
engage with the post, F(2, 678) = 3.33, p = .036, η
p
2
= .010. Participants in the periodic condition
were more likely to engage (M = 6.02, SD = 3.04) than those in the aggregate condition (M =
5.40, SD = 2.96), t(464) = 2.22, p = .027, d = .21. Participants in the periodic-sum condition (M
= 6.00, SD = 2.85) were also more likely to donate than those in the aggregate condition, t(453)
= 2.22, p = .027, d = .21. There was no difference between the two periodic conditions, t(439) =
.04, p = .97.
We tested whether the effect of donation framing on consumer engagement was mediated
by perceived commitment through a 3-cell multicategorical mediation (Hayes 2017, Model 4).
61
Contrasting the aggregate (0) and periodic (1) conditions, we tested the significance of
the measured mediator by calculating the standardized effects for 10,000 bootstrapped samples.
We found a statistically significant indirect effect of perceived commitment on engagement
(indirect effect = .43; 95% CI [.14, .73]; see Figure 2.4). Controlling for commitment, the direct
effect was non-significant (direct effect = .19, p = .43, 95% CI [-.28, .66]).
Contrasting the aggregate (0) and periodic-sum (1) conditions, we tested the significance
of the measured mediator by calculating the standardized effects for 10,000 bootstrapped
samples. Similarly, we found a statistically significant indirect effect of perceived commitment
on engagement (indirect effect = .49; 95% CI [.22, .77]; see Figure 2.4). Controlling for
commitment, the direct effect was non-significant (direct effect = .12, p = .63, 95% CI [-.36,
.59]).
FIGURE 2.4: STUDY 4 MEDIATION MODELS
NOTE.
*p < .05, **p < .01, *** p < .001
2.7.3 Discussion
Study 4 provides initial evidence for the mechanism of perceived commitment. We find
that participants are more likely to engage with companies framing their donations in periodic
62
(vs. aggregate) terms because they perceive those companies to be more committed to the
charitable cause. Moreover, we find that even when participants are aware of the total donation
amount with periodic donations (periodic-sum condition), participants perceive the companies as
more committed to the charitable cause and are more likely to engage with the company.
2.8 Study 5: Perceived Commitment Moderation
In Study 5, we directly manipulate the perceived commitment towards the prosocial
cause. We examine whether an additional cue of commitment (i.e., longer donation span)
attenuates the effect of periodic donations on perceived commitment and social media
engagement. If consumers infer that the periodic donations are an indicator of broader charitable
commitment (and by contrast, that an aggregate donation is an indicator that companies are less
committed to a cause), expanding the timeframe of donations should increase perceived
commitment for companies donating in aggregate. To that effect, we ask participants to judge
periodic (quarterly) and aggregate (annual) donations for a single-year or four-year span.
Varying a second indicator of temporal commitment (years donating) should lead participants to
infer a company is committed to a cause, whether the company’s donations are in aggregate or
periodic.
In this study participants assess a company’s donation behavior by viewing part of a
company’s financial report. The report indicates donations per year and fiscal quarter. Presenting
donations in this way allows us to address concerns that companies announce aggregated
donations to manage impressions instead of from a place of genuine commitment to a cause.
63
2.8.1 Methods
We recruited 1,001 participants (M
Age
= 39.99, 42.86% female) on Amazon MTurk. We
randomly assigned participants to one of four conditions in a 2 (donation framing: aggregate vs.
periodic) X 2 (donation span: one year vs. four years) between-subjects design. Following our
preregistration (https://aspredicted.org/F8R_HRJ), we excluded participants who failed either of
the two attention checks, leaving a sample of 798 for our final.
All participants saw part of a company’s expense report that indicates the company’s
donation schedule. Participants read about a company named Bell Electrek. More specifically,
we told participants, “Below is part of an expense report highlighting a company's charitable
contributions toward child hunger relief efforts. The expense report shows the donation schedule
by fiscal year (see Appendix D for materials). Participants in the periodic conditions saw that
Bell Electrek donated $25K per quarter (four times per fiscal year) while participants in the
aggregate conditions saw an annual donation of $100K (once at the end of the fiscal year).
Additionally, participants in the one-year conditions saw how much Bell Electrek donated over
the last year, while participants in the four-year conditions saw donations over the last four years.
Then, similar to Study 3, participants rated the company’ commitment (α = .97).
Moreover, we measured participants’ degree of engagement with the company by asking, “After
learning about Bell Electrek’s donation behaviors, are you more or less likely to:” 1) “Visit their
website”, 2) “Visit their social media pages”, and 3) “Visit the website of their partner charities”
(-5 = Much less likely, 5 = Much more likely). These items showed high reliability (α = .92), so
we created an engagement composite using the average of these items. We additionally measured
two exploratory dependent variables (see Appendix C for results).
64
2.8.2 Results
A 2 (donation framing: aggregate vs. periodic) x 2 (donation span: one year vs. four
years) ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of donation framing, F(1, 794) = 28.41, p <
.001, η
p
2
= .03, and a significant main effect of donation span, F(1, 794) = 123.48, p < .001, η
p
2
=.13. Most importantly, we found a significant interaction, F(1, 794) = 41.75, p < .001, η
p
2
=.05,
such that over a one year span, the company donating periodically (M = 5.59, SD = 1.14) was
perceived to be more committed than donating in aggregate (M = 3.62, SD = 1.61), t(794) =
14.44, p < .001, d = 1.45. We observed the same pattern when the company donated over a four-
year span, but the difference between conditions diminished significantly. Participants saw the
company that made periodic donations over a four-year period (M = 5.85, SD = 1.15) as more
committed than those that made donations in aggregate (M = 5.13, SD = 1.46) over the same
span, t(794) = 5.33, p < .001, d = .53 (see Figure 2.5).
A 2 (donation framing: aggregate vs. periodic) x 2 (donation span: one year vs. four
years) ANOVA yielded a non-significant main effect of donation framing, F(1, 794) = 2.58, p =
.11, η
p
2
= .003, and a significant main effect of donation span, F(1, 794) = 20.29, p < .001, η
p
2
=
.02. Most importantly, we found a significant interaction between the two variables, F(1, 794) =
7.32, p = .007, η
p
2
=.01. Over a one-year period, participants were more likely to engage with the
company that donated periodically (M = 1.27, SD = 1.89) than in aggregate (M = .18, SD = 2.19),
t(794) = 5.42, p < .001, d = .54. However, when the company donated across four years, there
was no difference in engagement between the periodic (M = 1.40, SD = 1.85) and aggregate
conditions (M = 1.08, SD = 2.06), t(794) = 1.61, p = .11 (see Figure 2.5).
65
We conducted a moderated mediation (Hayes 2017, Model 8) to test that perceived
commitment mediated the relationship of donation framing and donation span on consumer
engagement. We tested the significance of the mediator by calculating the standardized indirect
effect for 10,000 bootstrap samples. Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that commitment
mediated the interaction between donation framing and donation span on engagement (index = -
.70; 95% CI [-.97, -.47]). As hypothesized, perceived commitment mediated the effect of
donation framing on engagement when the donation was made for one year (indirect effect =
1.12; 95% CI [.86, 1.39]), however, the indirect effect is smaller for participants in the four-year
conditions (indirect effect = .41; 95% CI [.25, .60]).
FIGURE 2.5: STUDY 5 RESULTS
NOTE. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.
2.8.3 Discussion
If consumers perceive a company that make periodic (vs. aggregate) donations to be more
committed to a cause, then expanding the timeframe of donations from one year to four years
should increase this perceived commitment. Results from Study 5 are in line with this conjecture
showing participants a company’s donation in a four-year span increases commitment and
engagement likelihood. Therefore, we find causal evidence that consumers are more likely to
66
engage with a company with periodic donations due to the company’s perceived commitment to
a cause.
2.9 Study 6: Boundary Condition
In our final study, we examine an important boundary condition of our periodic donation
effect extremely small donations. Contrary to our findings, recent work shows that periodic
donations can harm perceptions of the donors (Basu 2021). Specifically, the author finds that
donors who make periodic (vs. aggregate) donations are perceived to be less morally
praiseworthy because people perceive donors donating periodically as making less of a sacrifice.
Reconciling the opposing effects, we identify a key difference between the materials used in
Basu (2021) and our studies. In the previous work, the author compares framing of donations
that total in small amounts (e.g., $250 donation, 400 cans, 35 boxes of toys). However, company
charitable donations typically happen on a much larger scale, which is largely why we observe
the periodic donation effect in our studies. Here, we extend the current literature by showing that
periodic effects are interpreted differently when the amount occurs at different scales. We predict
that when the total donation amounts are large, consumers will perceive a company more
positively with periodic framing. However, with smaller total donation amounts, we expect a
reversal such that people will perceive a company more positively with aggregate framing.
2.9.1 Methods
We recruited 1201 Prolific Academic participants (M
Age
= 36.24, 48.29% female).
Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions in a 2 (donation framing:
aggregate vs. periodic) x 2 (donation amount: large vs. small) between-subjects design. Per our
preregistration, only participants that correctly answered the attention check questions (N =
67
1,181) were included in the final analysis. The preregistration for this study can be found here:
https://aspredicted.org/SWP_H43.
Participants read that a fictional company named Alpha Manufacturing donated to a
charity focused on international disaster relief. Participants in the periodic-large condition read
that Alpha Manufacturing donated $10,000 every week, while the aggregate-large condition
donated $520,000. Participants in the periodic-small condition read that Alpha Manufacturing
donated $2 every week, while the aggregate-small condition donated $100.
Similar to Study 2, participants then rated the key dependent variable of company
perception (α = .95) and moral praiseworthiness (adapted from Basu 2021): “How morally
praiseworthy do you think the action of Alpha Manufacturing is?” (1 = Not at all, 7 =
Extremely).
2.9.2 Results
A 2 (donation framing: aggregate vs. periodic) x 2 (donation amount: large vs. small)
ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of donation framing, F(1, 1177) = 5.82, p = .016, η
p
2
=.004, and a significant main effect of donation amount, F(1, 1177) = 19.76, p < .001, η
p
2
=.02.
Importantly, we found a significant interaction F(1, 1177) = 9.27, p = .002, η
p
2
=.008, such that
when donation amounts were large, the periodic donation (M = 5.55, SD = 1.31) had greater
positive perceptions of the company than the aggregate donation (M = 5.29, SD = 1.30), t(1177)
= 2.41, p = .016, d = .20. However, when donation amounts were large, periodic framing (M =
4.58, SD = 1.43) had marginally less positive perceptions of the company than the aggregate
framing (M = 4.79, SD = 1.29), t(1177) = 1.89, p = .059, d = .15 (see Figure 2.6).
68
A 2 (donation framing: aggregate vs. periodic) x 2 (donation amount: large vs. small)
ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of donation framing, F(1, 1177) = 5.74, p = .017, η
p
2
=.005, and a significant main effect of donation amount F(1, 1177) = 29.87, p < .001, η
p
2
=.02.
Importantly, we found a significant interaction, F(1, 1177) = 14.79, p < .001, η
p
2
=.01. When
donation amounts were large, the periodic donation (M = 5.68, SD = 1.36) had greater positive
perceptions of the company than the aggregate donation (M = 5.38, SD = 1.43), t(1177) = 2.40, p
= .017, d = .20. However, replicating Basu (2021), when donation amounts were small, periodic
framing (M = 4.35, SD = 1.61) had lesser positive perceptions of the company than the aggregate
framing (M = 4.72, SD = 1.43), t(1177) = 3.05, p = .002, d = .25 (see Figure 2.6).
FIGURE 2.6: STUDY 6 RESULTS
NOTE. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.
2.9.3 Discussion
Study 6 demonstrates that periodic donations do not always impact company perceptions
positively. We find that when donation amounts are significantly smaller, periodic donations
negatively impact company perceptions, consistent with findings from Basu (2021).
69
2.10 General Discussion
Across seven studies, we find evidence that a periodic donation strategy (compared to an
aggregate donation strategy) increases positive impressions of companies and increases
consumer engagement. Periodic donations accomplish this by increasing the degree to which
consumers see companies as authentically committed to a cause. Studies 1A and 1B examined
the effect of periodic donation framing using two preregistered field experiments. Both studies
found that consumers were more likely to engage with online campaigns that were framed as
providing periodic donations. Study 2 supported the generalizability of the effect by employing a
stimulus sampling approach. Across donation causes, amounts, and phrasings, we found support
that periodic donation framing increased the degree to which consumers judged a company
positively. Study 3 tested our perceived commitment mechanism and found that it mediated the
relationship between periodic donation framing and company perceptions. Study 3 also ruled out
alternative mechanisms. In Study 4, we replicated the mechanism findings using a stronger three-
item measure of commitment and provided evidence of the robustness of periodic donations
when consumers knew about the total donation sum. Study 4 also replicated the findings of our
field studies by showing that consumers were more likely engage on social media with
authentically committed companies. In Study 5, we demonstrated that other temporal donation
cues moderated our effect. Finally, in Study 6 we provided evidence of an important boundary
condition. Namely, a donation must be sufficiently large to benefit from periodic framing.
Appendix
3.1.1 Chapter 1: Appendix A
This appendix reports additional findings that were not reported in the main document.
Methods and Results from Figure 1.1
We recruited 544 undergraduates from a private university (M
Age
= 22.75, 54.6% female).
Participants were asked, “To what extent would you feel 1) uncomfortable, 2) awkward, and 3)
uneasy talking about the following topics with your [close friends/distant acquaintances]?” (1 =
Not at all, 7 = Very much; adapted from Jiang, Hoegg, and Dahl 2013). The three items were
averaged to create a “discomfort” composite (α = .96). The topics were money issues, politics,
religion, sex, death, gossip, offensive jokes, relationship problems, health problems, and race.
The perceived discomfort was tested using a 2 (relationship type: close friends vs. distant
acquaintances) x 10 (list of topics) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). We found
significant main effects of relationship type (F(1, 10,317) = 5,695.23, p < .001), list of topics
(F(9, 10,317) = 102.15, p < .001), and interaction (F(9, 10,317) = 6.92, p < .001).
Overall, we found that people have varying levels of discomfort talking about sensitive
topics with close friends and distant acquaintances. First, people feel more uncomfortable talking
about sensitive topics with distant acquaintances than close friends. Second, people feel more
uncomfortable talking about offensive jokes, followed by money issues, sex, relationship
problems, health problems, politics, race, death, religion, and gossip.
[70]
Methods and Results of Social Richness
We recruited 100 Amazon MTurk participants via CloudResearch (M
Age
= 37.11, 35%
female). We used three items (adapted from Oh, Bailenson, and Welch 2018) to measure
perceived social richness of communicating 1) in person, 2) over a phone call, 3) via text, 4) via
email, 5) via Venmo, 6) via PayPal, and 7) via a standard bank app: “To what extent is
[communication method] a socially rich way to communicate with others?”, “To what extent
would you feel that you are in the presence of others when communicating [communication
method]?”, and To what extent would you feel connected with others when communicating
[communication method]” (1 = Not at all, 11 = Extremely; α = .94). The order in which
participants answered the social richness of each communication method was randomized. The
preregistration for this pilot test can be found here: https://aspredicted.org/ZXV_F8B
The perceived social richness was tested using a 7-cell (communication method: in-
person, phone call, text, email, Venmo, PayPal, bank app) repeated measures ANOVA. As
shown below, we found significant differences between the communication methods (F(6, 594)
= 239.50, p < .001). The table below displays the Bonferroni post-hoc analyses of the perceived
social richness of each communication method.
In summary, we found that each request method had different social richness levels that
are consistent with theories of social richness (Daft and Lengel 1986; Short, Williams, and
Christie, 1976). Specifically, in-person interactions were perceived to be most socially rich,
followed by phone calls, texts, email, Venmo and PayPal, and a standard bank app.
[71]
[72]
PERCEIVED SOCIAL RICHNESS BY COMMUNICATION METHODS
NOTE. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.
[73]
BONFERRONI POST-HOC ANALYSES OF PERCEIVED SOCIAL RICHNESS
Communication method
Communication method
Mean difference
SE
df
t
In-person (M =
10.19)
-
Phone
2.06
.26
594
8.05
-
Text
3.42
.26
594
13.36
-
Email
4.72
.26
594
18.46
-
Venmo
6.52
.26
594
25.49
-
PayPal
6.77
.26
594
26.45
-
Bank app
7.72
.26
594
30.19
Phone (M = 8.13)
-
Text
1.36
.26
594
5.31
-
Email
2.66
.26
594
10.41
-
Venmo
4.46
.26
594
17.44
-
PayPal
4.71
.26
594
18.40
-
Bank app
5.67
.26
594
22.14
Text (M = 6.77)
-
Email
1.30
.26
594
5.09
-
Venmo
3.10
.26
594
12.13
-
PayPal
3.35
.26
594
13.09
-
Bank app
4.31
.26
594
16.83
Email (M = 5.47)
-
Venmo
1.80
.26
594
7.03
-
PayPal
2.05
.26
594
8.00
-
Bank app
3.00
.26
594
11.73
Venmo (M = 3.67)
-
PayPal
.25
.26
594
.96
-
Bank app
1.20
.26
594
4.70
PayPal (M = 3.42)
-
Bank app (M = 2.46)
.96
.26
594
3.74
[74]
Additional Results from Study 1A
The tables below displays background descriptive statistics and logistic regression
models comparing 1) digital app versus text and 2) text versus in-person requests, respectively.
Despite the difference in perceived social richness between digital payment apps and text
requests, we found no significant association between relationship closeness and the choice of
request method when comparing digital app and text requests (ps > .05). However, we found a
significant negative association between relationship closeness and the choice of request method
when comparing text and in-person requests (β = -.27, SE = .08, z = -3.18, p = .001). This
association remained strong even after controlling for the amount of money that was owed (β = -
.28, SE = .09, z = -3.19, p = .001) and the number of days waited until requesting the money
back (β = -.27, SE = .08, z = -3.16, p = .002).
These patterns are consistent with H
1
,
such that with weaker social connections,
consumers chose to request through relatively less socially rich means, such as digital payment
apps and texts. However, they were more likely to request using more socially rich means such
as in person with stronger social ties.
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS (STUDY 1A)
N = 803
Mean Amount ($)
$284.62
Median Amount ($)
$100
Mean Days Until Request
24.88 days
Median Days Until Request
14 days
Choice of Request Method
[75]
In-person
57.66%
(463/803)
Phone Call
.37%
(3/803)
Text
22.54%
(181/803)
Digital Payment App
17.68%
(142/803)
Email
.88%
(7/803)
Other
.87%
(7/803)
LOGISTIC REGRESSION MODELS COMPARING DIGITAL APP VERSUS TEXT
REQUESTS (STUDY 1A)
Dependent variable: Choice of digital app (1) versus text (0) request
n = 323
Independent variable
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
Closeness
.06
(.11)
.07
(.11)
.06
(.11)
.07
(.11)
Amount of Money Owed
-.82*
(.38)
-.53
(.40)
Number of days waited
-.91*
(.36)
-.73
(.38)
NOTE. *p < .05, **p < .01, *** p < .001
[76]
LOGISTIC REGRESSION MODELS COMPARING TEXTS VERSUS IN PERSON
REQUESTS (STUDY 1A)
Dependent variable: Choice of text (1) versus in-person (0) request
n = 644
Independent variable
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
Closeness
-.27**
(.08)
-.28**
(.09)
-.27**
(.08)
-.27**
(.09)
Amount of Money Owed
.02
(.08)
.03
(.08)
Number of days waited
-.03
(.14)
-.05
(.15)
NOTE. *p < .05, **p < .01, *** p < .001
Additional Results from Study 1B
This has the descriptive statistics from Study 1B of how much money was owed, how
long they waited until requesting back owed money, and their choice of request method by
requestee relationship. Because of extreme outliers in the variables of the number of days
participants waited before requesting the money and how much their close friend or distant
acquaintance owed, we winsorized the top 10% of responses for those variables in the
descriptive statistics.
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS (STUDY 1B)
Close Friend
Distant Acquaintance
N = 192
178 (92.71%)
94 (48.96%)
Amount ($)
$168
$89.27
[77]
Close Friend
Distant Acquaintance
Days Until Request
27.73
21.97
Choice of Request Method
In-person
48.31%
(86/178)
41.49%
(39/94)
Phone Call
5.06%
(9/178)
6.38%
(6/94)
Text
33.70%
(60/178)
21.28%
(20/94)
Digital Payment App
11.24%
(20/178)
26.60%
(25/94)
Other
1.69%
(3/178)
4.25%
(4/94)
Additional Results from Studies 3, 4A, and 4B
PERCENTAGE OF DIGITAL APP REQUESTS BY REQUESTEE RELATIONSHIP AND
SCENARIOS (STUDIES 3, 4A, AND 4B)
Close
friend
Distant
acquaintance
p-value
Close
friend
Distant
acquaintance
p-value
35.94%
41.18%
p = .54
27.59%
47.06%
p = .02
31.08%
46.38%
p = .06
31.63%
44.88%
p = .006
[78]
Close
friend
Distant
acquaintance
p-value
Close
friend
Distant
acquaintance
p-value
Discomfort-Control
Discomfort-Decreased
38.26%
57.61%
p = .006
27.38%
36.90%
p = .19
32.93%
49.38%
p = .03
32%
48.84%
p = .02
34.12%
66.67%
p < .001
31.46%
39.56%
p = .26
35.46%
58.36%
p < .001
30.40%
41.76%
p = .006
Impersonal-Control
Impersonal-Averse
18.87%
29.91%
p = .06
7.38%
7.48%
p = .98
16.53%
42.27%
p < .001
13.51%
16.46%
p = .57
20.51%
54.21%
p < .001
8.33%
12.61%
p = .32
18.60%
41.74%
p < .001
9.73%
11.78%
p = .41
3.1.2 Chapter 1: Appendix B
This appendix has all the study stimuli.
STUDY SCENARIOS (STUDY 2)
Scenario
Amusement park
Imagine you and a group of people including Charlie, [one of your closest friends/one of your
distant acquaintances], have decided to go to an amusement park.
Because you bought everyone’s tickets, you paid the full price and everyone else will pay you
back their share which is $100 per person.
[79]
Scenario
Other than Charlie, everyone else paid you back the $100 they owed for the amusement park
tickets.
It has been a few days since Charlie borrowed the $100. However, Charlie still has not paid
you back the money.
Bar happy hour
Imagine you are attending a happy hour at a bar with a group of people including Charlie, [one
of your closest friends/one of your distant acquaintances].
As you are about to purchase your drink, Charlie informs you that they forgot their credit card
at home, and asks if you can pay for a few of their drinks which costs $20.
Charlie promises to pay you back as soon as possible.
It has been a couple of weeks since Charlie borrowed the $20. However, Charlie still has not
paid you back the money.
Car troubles
Imagine Charlie, [one of your closest friends/one of your distant acquaintances], is
experiencing some car troubles and they need to replace one of their tires.
Moreover, Charlie informs you that they don’t have enough money at the moment and asks if
they can borrow $80 to fix their car.
Charlie promises to pay you back as soon as they get paid in a few days.
It has been a week since Charlie borrowed the $80. However, Charlie still has not paid you
back the money.
Concert ticket
Imagine you are attending a concert with a group of people including Charlie, [one of your
closest friends/one of your distant acquaintances].
As you are about to purchase your ticket, Charlie informs you that they forgot their wallet in
the car, and asks if you can pay for their ticket which costs $50.
Charlie promises to pay you back as soon as possible.
[80]
Scenario
It has been a couple of weeks since Charlie borrowed the $50. However, Charlie still has not
paid you back the money.
Food
Imagine you are attending a dinner party with a group of people including Charlie, [one of
your closest friends/one of your distant acquaintances]. The group decides to order a couple of
appetizers and entrees to share.
After the meal, the group decides to evenly split the bill which comes out to $30 per person.
However, Charlie tells you that they forgot their wallet, and asks if you can cover their portion
of the bill.
Charlie promises to pay you back as soon as possible.
It has been a week since Charlie borrowed the $30. However, Charlie still has not paid you
back the money.
Gas money
Imagine you and a group of people including Charlie, [one of your closest friends/one of your
distant acquaintances], recently went on a road trip for a few days.
Because you drove your car, you ask everyone if they can pitch in for gas which is $25 per
person.
While everyone pitched in, Charlie has not given you the $25 they owed for gas.
It has been a few days since Charlie borrowed the $25. However, Charlie still has not paid you
back the money.
Housing
Imagine you and a group of people including Charlie, [one of your closest friends/one of your
distant acquaintances], have rented an Airbnb for a cabin trip in the mountains.
Because you were in charge of booking the cabin, you paid the full price and everyone else
will pay you back their share which is $75 per person.
Other than Charlie, everyone else paid you back the $75 they owed for the Airbnb.
[81]
Scenario
It has been a couple of weeks since Charlie borrowed the $75. However, Charlie still has not
paid you back the money.
Movie ticket
Imagine you are going to the theaters with a group of people including Charlie, [one of your
closest friends/one of your distant acquaintances].
As you are about to purchase your ticket, Charlie informs you that they forgot their wallet at
home and asks if you can pay for their ticket which costs $15.
Charlie promises to pay you back as soon as possible.
It has been a couple of days since Charlie borrowed the $15 for their ticket. However, Charlie
still has not paid you back the money.
Uber ride
Imagine you and a group of people including Charlie, [one of your closest friends/one of your
distant acquaintances], are planning to visit someone's house. You decide to call an Uber for
everyone to share.
Because you called the Uber, paid the full price and everyone else will pay you back their
share, which is $10 per person.
Other than Charlie, everyone else paid you back the $10 they owed for the Uber ride.
It has been a couple of days since Charlie borrowed the $10. However, Charlie still has not
paid you back the money.
STUDY SCENARIO (STUDY 5)
Close friend/Distant acquaintance
Vendor
Imagine that there is a local farmers' market
that you go to once a week on your way
home.
During one visit, you find out that a pottery
vendor is selling a set of dishes that you really
like for $100. You decide to buy them to take
Imagine that there is a local farmers' market
that you go to once a week on your way
home.
During one visit, you find out that a pottery
vendor is selling a set of dishes for sale
for $100 that you really like. You decide to
[82]
Close friend/Distant acquaintance
Vendor
home.
On your way home, you run into Charlie, [one
of your closest friends/one of your distant
acquaintances]. You and Charlie start talking
about the new set of dishes you just
purchased.
While showing Charlie the set of dishes,
they accidentally drop the set breaking all the
dishes.
Charlie apologizes and promises to pay back
the cost of the dishes as soon as possible.
It has been a week, however, Charlie still has
not paid you back for the broken set of dishes.
There are three ways to request the money
from Charlie: using Venmo, requesting in
person, or using a standard bank app.
Venmo: Venmo is a digital payment service
that allows users to request money through its
phone app.
In person: You can request the money
from Charlie face to face (assume you see
Charlie once a week and the money will be
paid through a digital payment method).
A standard bank app: You can use another
standard bank app on your phone that allows
users to request money through a phone app.
How would you request the $100 from
Charlie, [one of your closest friends/one of
your distant acquaintances]?
buy them to take home.
However, when you arrive home you discover
that some of the dishes are broken.
The vendor has a policy that if any of the
dishes are broken in the box when you get
home, you can either bring them back the next
time you come and request a refund in person,
or you can take a photo of the broken dishes
and request the refund electronically.
It has been a week since you purchased the
broken set of dishes from the pottery vendor.
There are three ways to request the money
from the vendor: using Venmo, requesting in
person, or using a standard bank app.
Venmo: Venmo is a digital payment service
that allows users to request money through its
phone app.
In person: You can request the money
from the vendor face to face (assume you go
to this farmers market once a week and the
money will be paid through a digital payment
method).
A standard bank app: You can use another
standard bank app on your phone that allows
users to request money through a phone app.
How would you request the $100 from the
pottery vendor?
[83]
3.2.1 Chapter 2: Appendix C
This appendix reports additional findings that were not reported in the main document.
Results from Study 3
MEANS OF DEPENDENT VARIABLES BY SCENARIO AND CONDITIONS
(STUDY 3)
Scenario
Dependent
Variable
Periodic
Condition
Aggregate
Condition
p-value
Environment
Donation
Company
Perception
5.53
5.18
.040
Perceived
Commitment
5.49
4.88
.001
Perceived Costs
4.39
3.98
.032
Perceived
Benefits
5.56
5.25
.031
Hunger Donation
Company
Perception
5.39
4.89
.003
Perceived
Commitment
4.91
4.28
.004
Perceived Costs
3.91
3.72
.35
Perceived
Benefits
5.35
5.22
.39
Refugee Donation
Company
Perception
4.81
4.57
.23
Scenario
Dependent
Variable
Periodic
Condition
Aggregate
Condition
p-value
Perceived
Commitment
4.49
4.31
.43
Perceived Costs
2.99
3.23
.24
Perceived
Benefits
4.66
4.91
.15
Collapsed
Company
Perception
5.27
4.88
< .001
Perceived
Commitment
5.00
4.49
< .001
Perceived Costs
3.81
3.64
.25
Perceived
Benefits
5.23
5.13
.33
Results from Study 4
In Study 4, we measured exploratory variables of perceived impact and donation
likelihood. Perceived impact was measured by the following item: “How impactful will this
company's involvement be in addressing child hunger?” (1 = Not at all, 5 = Extremely).
Donation likelihood was measured the following item: “If Barksdale Materials shared a link to
donate directly to one of the charities they are partnering with, how likely would you be to
donate?” (0 = Not at all, 10 = Extremely likely). The table below displays the series of t-tests for
each variable.
MEANS OF EXPLORATORY VARIABLES BY CONDITIONS
(STUDY 4)
[84]
[85]
Exploratory
Variable
Condition
(Mean)
Condition
(Mean)
t-tests
Perceived
Impact
Periodic
(3.73)
-
Aggregate
(3.46)
t(464) = 2.72, p = .007
-
Periodic-sum
(3.62)
t(439) = 1.08, p = .28
Aggregate
-
Periodic-sum
t(453) = 1.69, p = .092
Donation
Likelihood
Periodic
(5.28)
-
Aggregate
(4.91)
t(464) = 1.32, p = .19
-
Periodic-sum
(5.38)
t(439) = .34, p = .73
Aggregate
-
Periodic-sum
t(453) = 1.73, p = .085
Results from Study 5
In Study 5, we measured exploratory variables of perceived impact and donation
likelihood. Perceived impact was measured by averaging the following items: “This company’s
donation behavior will: 1) feed many people, 2) have a noticeable impact in their community,
and 3) significantly reduce child hunger (1 = Strongly disagree, 7 = Strongly agree) (α = .93).
Donation likelihood was measured averaging the following items: 1) If Bell Electrek shared a
[86]
link to donate directly to one of the charities they are partnering with, how likely would you be
to donate?” (0 = Not at all, 10 = Extremely likely), 2) If Bell Electrek offered donation matching
(in which they donate a dollar for every dollar you donate) for donating to one of their partnering
charities, how likely would you be to donate?” (0 = Not at all, 10 = Extremely likely), and 3) If
you decided to donate to a child hunger relief organization, would you be more likely to donate
once to a charity or sign up for a monthly donation?” (-5 = I would definitely only donate once, 5
= I would definitely sign up for monthly donations). The last item was recoded so that the values
of the anchors matched the first two items. The table below displays the post hoc analyses for
each variable.
POST HOC ANALYSES OF EXPLORATORY VARIABLES BY CONDITIONS
(STUDY 5)
Exploratory
Variable
Donation
Framing-
Donation Span
Donation
Framing-
Donation Span
Mean
difference
SE
df
t
p
Perceived
Impact
Periodic-One
-
Aggregate-One
.54
.13
794
4.08
<
.001
Periodic-Four
-
Aggregate-Four
.16
.13
794
1.19
.24
Donation
Likelihood
Periodic-One
-
Aggregate-One
.13
.24
794
.57
.57
Periodic-Four
-
Aggregate-Four
-.08
.24
794
-.33
.74
Methods and Results of Additional Study
We recruited 602 Prolific Academic participants (M
Age
= 34.61, 49.17% female).
Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions in a 2-cell (donation framing:
aggregate vs. periodic) between-subjects design. Each participant was then randomly assigned to
view one of three scenarios (environmental, hunger, social activism donations; see Appendix D).
The donation amount and donation cause were different across the three scenarios. In accordance
with our preregistration (https://aspredicted.org/9NN_6QN), only participants that answered the
attention check questions correctly (N = 579) were included in the analysis.
Participants read that a fictional company pledged to donate to a certain prosocial cause.
We describe the environment scenario below, but all scenarios follow a similar pattern.
Participants in the environment scenario read that a company named Smith Enterprise recently
pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to protect the environment. The donation pledge
read,
“Smith Enterprise recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant
amount of good for the environment. We pledge to donate a [aggregate condition: one-
time donation of $1.2 million in 2023/periodic condition: $100,000 each month between
January 2023 and December 2023] to non-profit environmental organizations protecting
the environment and wildlife.”
Participants then rated the key dependent variable of company perception using three
items: 1) How much do you admire [company name]?”, 2) “How favorably do you view
[company name]?”, and 3) How positively do you view [company name]?(1 = Not at all, 7 =
Very much). The three items were averaged to create a “company perception” composite (α =
.97). We also measured an exploratory variable of perceived authenticity using three items: 1)
[87]
[88]
“How authentic is [company name]’s support for the cause?”, 2) “How sincere is [company
name]’s donation?”, and 3) “How genuine is [company name]’s donation?” (1 = Not at all, 7 =
Very)
Following our preregistered analysis, we regressed company perception on the framing of
donation pledges, including a fixed effect of scenarios. As predicted, participants in the periodic
condition (M = 5.17, SD = 1.42) had greater positive perceptions of the company than the
aggregate condition (M = 4.50, SD = 1.64), t(575) = 5.24, p < .001. The pattern of results is
consistent when looking at each scenario separately (see table below).
Similar to our key dependent variable, we regressed perceived authenticity on the
framing of donation pledges, including a fixed effect of scenarios. Consistent with previous
results, participants in the periodic condition (M = 5.28, SD = 1.31) had greater positive
perceptions of the company than the aggregate condition (M = 4.75, SD = 1.44), t(575) = 4.48, p
< .001. The pattern of results is consistent when looking at each scenario separately (see table
below).
MEANS OF DEPENDENT VARIABLES BY SCENARIO AND CONDITIONS
Scenario
Dependent
Variable
Periodic
Condition
Aggregate
Condition
p-value
Environment
Donation
Company
Perception
5.29
4.77
.007
Perceived
Authenticity
5.38
4.82
.004
[89]
Scenario
Dependent
Variable
Periodic
Condition
Aggregate
Condition
p-value
Hunger Donation
Company
Perception
5.45
4.98
.026
Perceived
Authenticity
5.50
5.04
.013
Social Activism
Donation
Company
Perception
4.79
3.86
< .001
Perceived
Authenticity
4.96
4.45
.017
3.2.2 Chapter 2: Appendix D
This appendix has all the study stimuli.
GIFTAMEAL FIELD EXPERIMENT ADVERTISEMENTS (STUDY 1B)
[90]
[91]
STUDY SCENARIOS (STUDIES 2, 3, & APPENDIX C)
SCENARIOS
STUDY 2
Education cause
ANY Corporation is an American company that designs and sells children's apparel, with over
800 stores across North America, Europe, and Asia.
Imagine that ANY Corporation recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to
support children’s education and public schools. This was their pledging statement:
“ANY Corporation recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount
of good. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a one-time donation of $1.2
million/periodic condition: $90,000 every month between January 2023 and December 2023]
to non-profit organizations that support children’s education and public schools.”
Environmental cause
Smith Enterprise is an American company that sells various equipment for outdoor excursions
such as travel equipment, camping gear, clothing, and sports equipment.
Imagine that Smith Enterprise recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to support
wildlife and environmental conservation. This was their pledging statement:
Smith Enterprise recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount of
good. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: $2.5 million in 2023/periodic condition:
$200,000 every month between January 2023 and December 2023] to non-profit organizations
that support wildlife and environmental conservation.”
[92]
SCENARIOS
Gender equality cause
FYA Jewelry is an American retailer and producer of jewelry that is known for their
customizable bracelets, designer rings, earrings, and necklaces. The company markets its
products in more than 50 countries on six continents.
Imagine that FYA Jewelry recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to support
gender equality. This was their pledging statement:
FYA Jewelry recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount of
good. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a one-time donation of $300,000 in
2023/periodic condition: $24,000 every month between January 2023 and December 2023] to
non-profit organizations that support gender equality.”
Health 1 cause
Alpha Manufacturing is an American company that manufactures various parts for
automobiles, home appliances, and textiles.
Imagine that Alpha Manufacturing recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to
support individuals, families, and caregivers affected by Alzheimer’s disease. This was their
pledging statement:
Alpha Manufacturing recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant
amount of good. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a one-time donation of $600,000
in 2023/periodic condition: $45,000 every month between January 2023 and December 2023]
to non-profit organizations that support individuals, families, and caregivers affected by
Alzheimer’s disease.”
Health 2 cause
Zeta LLC is a multinational investment bank and financial services company that provides
various financial services to different clients.
[93]
SCENARIOS
Imagine that Zeta LLC recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to support
individuals, families, and caregivers affected by Parkinson’s disease. This was their pledging
statement:
Zeta LLC recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount of good.
We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a total donation of $2 million in 2023/periodic
condition: $150,000 every month between January 2023 and December 2023] to non-profit
organizations that support individuals, families, and caregivers affected by Parkinson’s
disease.”
Hunger cause
YNA Corporation is an American fashion retailer company that sells accessories, beauty
products, and clothing for people of all ages, with over 700 stores across North America,
Europe, and Asia.
Imagine that YNA Corp recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to support the
ending of world hunger. This was their pledging statement:
YNA Corp recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount of
good. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a total donation of $750,000 in
2023/periodic condition: $60,000 every month between January 2023 and December 2023] to
non-profit organizations that support the ending of world hunger.”
Poverty cause
Beta Tech is an American technology company that focuses on online advertising, cloud
computing, e-commerce, and consumer electronics.
Imagine that Beta Tech recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to support the
ending of global poverty. This was their pledging statement:
“Beta Tech recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount of good.
[94]
SCENARIOS
We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: $1 million in 2023/periodic condition: $80,000
every month between January 2023 and December 2023] to non-profit organizations that
support the ending of global poverty.”
Refugee cause
Johnson & Williams is an American electronics company that sells various office supplies
such as computers, printers, and laptops to firms across the globe.
Imagine that Johnson & Williams recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to
support war refugees and provide relief. This was their pledging statement:
“Johnson & Williams recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant
amount of good. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a total donation of $1.5 million in
2023/periodic condition: $120,000 every month between January 2023 and December 2023] to
non-profit organizations that support war refugees and provide relief.”
Social activism cause
J&L is an American food company that is one of the biggest food suppliers to restaurants and
groceries across the globe.
Imagine that J&L recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to support and protect
human rights. This was their pledging statement:
“J&L recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount of good. We
pledge to donate [aggregate condition: $200,000 in 2023/periodic condition: $16,000 every
month between January 2023 and December 2023] to non-profit organizations that support and
protect human rights.”
STUDY 3
Environmental cause
[95]
SCENARIOS
Smith Enterprise is an American company that sells various equipment for outdoor excursions
such as travel equipment, camping gear, clothing, and sports equipment.
Imagine that Smith Enterprise recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to protect
the environment. This was their pledging statement:
“Smith Enterprise recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount of
good for the environment. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a one-time donation of
$1.2 million in 2023/periodic condition: $100,000 every month between January 2023 and
December 2023] to non-profit environmental organizations protecting the environment and
wildlife.”
Hunger cause
YNA Corporation is an American fashion retailer company that sells accessories, beauty
products, and clothing for people of all ages, with over 700 stores across North America,
Europe, and Asia.
Imagine that YNA Corporation recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to
support the ending of World Hunger. This was their pledging statement:
“YNA Corporation recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount
of good to end world hunger. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a one-time donation
of $720,000 in 2023/periodic condition: $60,000 every month between January 2023 and
December 2023] to international non-profit organizations that aim to create a hunger-free
world.
Refugee cause
Johnson & Williams is an American electronics company that sells various office supplies
[96]
SCENARIOS
such as computers, printers, and laptops to firms across the globe.
Imagine that Johnson & Williams recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to aid
war and refugee relief. This was their pledging statement:
“Johnson & Williams recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant
amount of good for aid refugees across the globe. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a
one-time donation of $60,000 in 2023/periodic condition: $5,000 every month between
January 2023 and December 2023] to non-profit organizations helping and protecting war
refugees.”
Environmental cause
Smith Enterprise is an American company that sells various equipment for outdoor excursions
such as travel equipment, camping gear, clothing, and sports equipment.
Imagine that Smith Enterprise recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to protect
the environment. This was their pledging statement:
“Smith Enterprise recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount of
good for the environment. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a one-time donation of
$1.2 million in 2023/periodic condition: $100,000 each month between January 2023 and
December 2023] to non-profit environmental organizations protecting the environment and
wildlife.”
Hunger cause
Greater Goods is an American chain store of bakery-café fast-casual restaurants with over
2,000 locations across the United States and Canada.
Imagine that Greater Goods recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to support
[97]
SCENARIOS
the ending of World Hunger. This was their pledging statement:
“Greater Goods recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount of
good to end world hunger. We pledge to donate [aggregate condition: a one-time donation of
$600,000 this year/periodic condition: $50,000 per month from June 2022 to May 2023] to
international non-profit organizations that aim to create a hunger-free world.
Social activism cause
YNA Corporation is an American fashion retailer company that sells accessories, beauty
products, and clothing for people of all ages, with over 700 stores across North America,
Europe, and Asia.
Imagine that YNA Corporation recently pledged to donate a portion of their earnings to the
Black Lives Matter social movement. This was their pledging statement:
“YNA Corporation recognizes that we can use part of our earnings to do a significant amount
of good for the Black Lives Matter social movement. We pledge to donate [aggregate
condition: a one-time donation of $720,000 in 2023/periodic condition: $60,000 every month
starting in January 2023 and going until December 2023] to non-profit organizations helping
and advocating for various policy changes and anti-racism.
PERCEIVED COST AND BENEFITS MEASURES (STUDY 3)
Perceived costs measures (7-point Likert Scale; 1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree)
1. <r> The cost of this donation is trivial to [company name]
2. The cost this donation is a lot of money
3. <r> [company name] would barely notice the cost of this donation.
[98]
Perceived benefits measures (7-point Likert Scale; 1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree)
1. Non-profit organizations would miss out on many benefits if [company name] did not
donate.
2. Non-profit organizations would benefit a lot from the donation.
3. <r> The donation would not be beneficial to non-profit organizations.
STUDY STIMULI: PERIODIC CONDITION (LEFT), PERIODIC-SUM CONDITION
(CENTER), AND AGGREGATE CONDITION (RIGHT) (STUDY 4)
STUDY STIMULI: PERIODIC-ONE YEAR CONDITION (FIRST), AGGREGATE-ONE
YEAR CONDITION (SECOND), PERIODIC-FOUR YEAR CONDITION (THIRD),
AGGREGATE-FOUR YEAR CONDITION (FOURTH) (STUDY 5)
Periodic-One Year
[99]
Aggregate-One Year
Periodic-Four Year
Aggregate-Four Year
100
References/Bibliography/Works Cited
Aknin, Lara B., Michael I. Norton, and Elizabeth W. Dunn (2009), "From wealth to well-being?
Money matters, but less than people think." The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4 (6),
523-527.
Alberhasky, M. and Amit Kumar (2021), “A Venmo Effect on Relationships: Electronic
Payment Makes Social Relations More Transactional and Experiences Less Enjoyable.”,
working paper.
Amar, Moty, Dan Ariely, Shahar Ayal, Cynthia E. Cryder, and Scott I. Rick (2011), "Winning
the battle but losing the war: The psychology of debt management." Journal of Marketing
Research 48, no. SPL: S38-S50.
Aron, Arthur, Elaine N. Aron, and Danny Smollan (1992). "Inclusion of other in the self scale
and the structure of interpersonal closeness." Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 63 (4): 596.
Atlas, Stephen A., and Daniel M. Bartels (2018). "Periodic pricing and perceived contract
benefits." Journal of Consumer Research, 45(2), 350-364.
Baker, Wayne E. and Jason B. Jimerson (1992), “The Sociology of Money,” American
Behavioral Scientist, 35(6), 67893.
Banerjee, Abhijit V., and Esther Duflo (2007). "The economic lives of the poor." Journal of
Economic Perspectives, 21 (1): 141-168.
101
Barasch, Alixandra, and Jonah Berger (2014). "Broadcasting and narrowcasting: How audience
size affects what people share." Journal of Marketing Research, 51 (3), 286-299.
Barasch, Alixandra, Emma E. Levine, Jonathan Z. Berman, and Deborah A. Small (2014).
"Selfish or selfless? On the signal value of emotion in altruistic behavior." Journal of
personality and social psychology, 107(3), 393.
Batson, C. Daniel, Judy G. Batson, Jacqueline K. Slingsby, Kevin L. Harrell, Heli M. Peekna,
and R. Matthew Todd. "Empathic joy and the empathy-altruism hypothesis." Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 413.
Basu, Shankha (2021). "Framing an altruistic action in periodic (versus aggregate) terms reduces
people's moral evaluation of the act and the actor." Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 97, 104205.
Belk, Russell W. (1988), "Possessions and the extended self." Journal of Consumer Research, 15
(2), 139-168.
Belk, Russell W. and Melanie Wallendorf (1990), “The Sacred Meanings of Money,Journal of
Economic Psychology, 11(1), 3567.
Berman, Jonathan Z., Emma E. Levine, Alixandra Barasch, and Deborah A. Small (2015). "The
Braggart's dilemma: On the social rewards and penalties of advertising prosocial
behavior." Journal of Marketing Research, 52 (1), 90-104.
Berman, Jonathan Z., Emma E. Levine, Alixandra Barasch, and Deborah A. Small (2015). "The
Braggart's dilemma: On the social rewards and penalties of advertising prosocial
behavior." Journal of Marketing Research, 52(1), 90-104.
102
Beshears, John, James J. Choi, David Laibson, Brigitte C. Madrian, and Katherine L. Milkman
(2015), "The effect of providing peer information on retirement savings decisions." The
Journal of Finance, 70 (3), 1161-1201.
Best, Raynor de (2021). “Topic: Mobile Payments Worldwide.” Statista. Accessed January 1,
2022. https://www.statista.com/topics/4872/mobile-payments-
worldwide/#:~:text=In%202023%2C%20it%20is%20estimated,950%20million%20users
%20in%202019.
Braithwaite, Tom, Camilla Hall, Tracy Alloway, Tim Bradshaw, Sarah Mishkin, and Richard
Waters (2014). “Apple wages war on the wallet, Financial Times, September 12, 2014,
Brown, Jacqueline Johnson, and Peter H. Reingen (1987), “Social Ties and Word-of-Mouth
Referral Behavior,” Journal of Consumer Research, 14(3), 350.
Brown, Tom J., and Peter A. Dacin (1997). "The Company and the Product: Corporate
Associations and Consumer Product Responses." Journal of Marketing, 61(1), 68-84.
Bursztyn, Leonardo, Florian Ederer, Bruno Ferman, and Noam Yuchtman (2014),
"Understanding mechanisms underlying peer effects: Evidence from a field experiment
on financial decisions." Econometrica, 82 (4), 1273-1301.
Carlin, Bruce I., Li Jiang, and Stephen A. Spiller (2018). "Millennial-style learning: Search
intensity, decision making, and information sharing." Management Science, 64 (7), 3313-
3330.
103
Cialdini, Robert B., John T. Cacioppo, Rodney Bassett, and John A. Miller (1978). "Low-ball
procedure for producing compliance: commitment then cost." Journal of personality and
Social Psychology, 36(5), 463.
Clark, Margaret S., and Judson Mills (1979), “Interpersonal attraction in exchange and
communal relationships,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(1), 12.
Clark, Margaret S., and Judson Mills (1993). "The difference between communal and exchange
relationships: What it is and is not." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19 (6),
684-691.
Clark, Margaret S., and Judson Mills (2012), "A theory of communal (and exchange)
relationships." Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology, 2, 232-250.
Clayton, Maya, José Liñares-Zegarra, and John OS Wilson (2015), "Does debt affect health?
Cross country evidence on the debt-health nexus." Social Science & Medicine 130, 51-58.
Corfman, Kim P., and Donald R. Lehmann (1987). "Models of cooperative group decision
making and relative influence: An experimental investigation of family purchase
decisions." Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (1), 1-13.
Critcher, Clayton R., and David Dunning (2014). "Thinking about others versus another: Three
reasons judgments about collectives and individuals differ." Social and Personality
Psychology Compass, 8(12), 687-698.
Cui, Yanli, Elizabeth S. Trent, Pauline M. Sullivan, and Grace N. Matiru (2003). "Cause‐related
marketing: how generation Y responds." International journal of retail & distribution
management, 31(6), 310-320.
104
D'souza, Giles, and Ram C. Rao (1995). "Can repeating an advertisement more frequently than
the competition affect brand preference in a mature market?." Journal of
Marketing, 59(2), 32-42.
Daft, Richard L., and Robert H. Lengel (1986). "Organizational information requirements, media
richness and structural design." Management science, 32 (5), 554-571.
De La Rosa, Wendy, and Stephanie M. Tully (2022). "The impact of payment frequency on
consumer spending and subjective wealth perceptions." Journal of Consumer Research,
48(6), 991-1009.
Dean, Dwane Hal (2003). "Consumer perception of corporate donations effects of company
reputation for social responsibility and type of donation." Journal of Advertising, 32(4),
91-102.
Ellen, Pam Scholder, Deborah J. Webb, and Lois A. Mohr (2006). "Building corporate
associations: Consumer attributions for corporate socially responsible programs." Journal
of the academy of Marketing Science, 34(2), 47-157.
English, Tammy, and Serena Chen (2011). "Self-concept consistency and culture: The
differential impact of two forms of consistency." Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 37(6), 838-849.
Ferber, Robert, and Lucy Chao Lee (1974). "Husband-wife influence in family purchasing
behavior." Journal of Consumer Research, 1 (1), 43-50.
Fiske, Alan P (1992). "The four elementary forms of sociality: framework for a unified theory of
social relations." Psychological Review, 99 (4), 689.
105
Fitch, Chris, Sarah Hamilton, Paul Bassett, and Ryan Davey (2011), "The relationship between
personal debt and mental health: a systematic review." Mental Health Review Journal.
Foreh, Mark R., and Sonya Grier (2003). "When is honesty the best policy? The effect of stated
company intent on consumer skepticism." Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13(3), 349-
356.
Fowler, James H., and Nicholas A. Christakis (2008), "Dynamic spread of happiness in a large
social network: longitudinal analysis over 20 years in the Framingham Heart
Study." BMJ, 337.
Frederick, Shane, and Baruch Fischoff (1998). Scope (in)sensitivity in elicited valuations.” Risk
Decision and Policy, 3, 109-123
Furnham, Adrian (1984), "Many sides of the coin: The psychology of money usage." Personality
and individual Differences, 5 (5), 501-509.
Furnham, Adrian, Emma Wilson, and Kate Telford (2012), "The meaning of money: The
validation of a short money-types measure." Personality and Individual
Differences, 52(6), 707-711.
Garbinsky, Emily N., and Joe J. Gladstone (2019). "The consumption consequences of couples
pooling finances." Journal of Consumer Psychology, 29 (3), 353-369.
Garcia-Rada, Ximena, Mary Steffel, Elanor F. Williams, and Michael I. Norton (2021).
"Consumers Value Effort over Ease When Caring for Close Others." Journal of
Consumer Research.
106
Gershon, Rachel, Cynthia Cryder, and Leslie K. John (2020). "Why prosocial referral incentives
work: The interplay of reputational benefits and action costs." Journal of Marketing
Research 57 (1), 156-172.
Gershon, Rachel, and Rosanna K. Smith (2020). "Twice-told tales: Self-repetition decreases
observer assessments of performer authenticity." Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 118(2), 307.
Gourville, John T (1998). "Pennies-a-day: The effect of temporal reframing on transaction
evaluation." Journal of Consumer Research, 24(4), 395-408.
Granovetter, Mark S. (1973), “The strength of weak ties,” American Journal of Sociology, 78(6),
1360-1380.
Hart, Einav, Eric M. VanEpps, and Maurice E. Schweitzer (2021), "The (better than expected)
consequences of asking sensitive questions." Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 162, 136-154.
Hayes, Andrew F (2017), Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process
analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford publications.
Hershfield, Hal E., Abigail B. Sussman, Rourke L. O’Brien, and Christopher J. Bryan (2015),
"Leveraging psychological insights to encourage the responsible use of consumer
debt." Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10 (6), 749-752.
Heyman, James, and Dan Ariely (2004). "Effort for payment: A tale of two markets."
Psychological Science, 15 (11), 787-793.
107
Huang, Liang, Anastasiya Pocheptsova Ghosh, Ruoou Li, and Elise Chandon Ince (2020). "Pay
Me with Venmo: Effect of Service Providers’ Decisions to Adopt P2P Payment Methods
on Consumer Evaluations." Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 5(3),
271-281.
Ivanic, Aarti S., Jennifer R. Overbeck, and Joseph C. Nunes (2011), "Status, race, and money:
The impact of racial hierarchy on willingness to pay." Psychological science, 22 (12),
1557-1566.
Jaroszewicz, Ania (2020), "It Does Hurt to Ask: Theory and Evidence on Informal Help-
Seeking." PhD diss., Carnegie Mellon University.
Jiang, Lan, JoAndrea Hoegg, and Darren W. Dahl (2013). "Consumer reaction to unearned
preferential treatment." Journal of Consumer Research, 40 (3), 412-427.
Judd, Charles M., Jacob Westfall, and David A. Kenny (2012). "Treating stimuli as a random
factor in social psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely
ignored problem." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 103 (1), 54.
Kahneman, Daniel, Jack L. Knetsch, and Richard H. Thaler (1990). "Experimental tests of the
endowment effect and the Coase theorem." Journal of Political Economy, 98(6), 1325-
1348.
Kahneman, Daniel, and Angus Deaton (2010), "High income improves evaluation of life but not
emotional well-being." Proceedings of the national academy of sciences, 107 (38) 16489-
16493.
108
Kelley, Harold H (1973). "The processes of causal attribution." American Psychologist, 28(2),
107.
Kiesler, Sara, Jane Siegel, and Timothy W. McGuire (1984), “Social psychological aspects of
computer-mediated communication,” American Psychologist, 39(10), 1123.
Klein, Jill, and Niraj Dawar. "Corporate social responsibility and consumers' attributions and
brand evaluations in a productharm crisis." International Journal of research in
Marketing 21, no. 3 (2004): 203-217.
Krackhardt, David, N. Nohria, and B. Eccles (2003), “The strength of strong ties,” Networks in
the knowledge economy, 82.
Kramer, Mark R., and Michael E. Porter (2011). "Creating shared value." Harvard Business
Review, 89(12), 62-77.
Kraus, Michael W., Serena Chen, and Dacher Keltner (2011). "The power to be me: Power
elevates self-concept consistency and authenticity." Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 47(5), 974-980.
Krueger, David, W. (1991), “Money meanings and madness: A psychoanalytic perspective,”
Psychoanalytic Review, 78(2), 209-224.
Kruger, Justin, Nicholas Epley, Jason Parker, and Zhi-Wen Ng (2005), "Egocentrism over e-
mail: Can we communicate as well as we think?." Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 89 (6), 925.
109
Lee, Samuel and Petra Persson (2016), “Financing from Family and Friends,” Review of
Financial Studies, 29(9), 234186.
Lev, Baruch, Christine Petrovits, and Suresh Radhakrishnan (2010). "Is doing good good for
you? How corporate charitable contributions enhance revenue growth." Strategic
Management Journal, 31(2), 182-200.
Lichtenstein, Donald R., Minette E. Drumwright, and Bridgette M. Braig (2004). "The effect of
corporate social responsibility on customer donations to corporate-supported
nonprofits." Journal of Marketing, 68(4), 16-32.
Lin-Healy, Fern, and Deborah A. Small (2013). "Nice guys finish last and guys in last are nice:
The clash between doing well and doing good." Social Psychological and Personality
Science, 4(6), 692-698.
Luft, Joseph (1957), "Monetary value and the perception of persons." The Journal of Social
Psychology, 46 (2), 245-251.
Lydon, John E., David W. Jamieson, and John G. Holmes (1997). "The meaning of social
interactions in the transition from acquaintanceship to friendship." Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 73 (3), 536.
Mazar, Nina, Daniel Mochon, and Dan Ariely (2018). "If you are going to pay within the next 24
hours, press 1: automatic planning prompt reduces credit card delinquency." Journal of
Consumer Psychology, 28 (3), 466-476.
110
McGraw, A. Peter, Philip E. Tetlock, and Orie V. Kristel (2003), "The limits of fungibility:
Relational schemata and the value of things." Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (2),
219-229.
Mogilner, Cassie (2010). "The pursuit of happiness: Time, money, and social
connection." Psychological Science, 21 (9), 1348-1354.
Mogilner, Cassie, and Michael I. Norton (2016), "Time, money, and happiness." Current
Opinion in Psychology, 10, 12-16.
Mohr, Lois A., and Deborah J. Webb (2005). "The effects of corporate social responsibility and
price on consumer responses." Journal of Consumer Affairs, 39(1), 121-147.
Monga, Ashwani, and Rajesh Bagchi (2012). "Years, months, and days versus 1, 12, and 365:
The influence of units versus numbers." Journal of Consumer Research, 39(1), 185-198.
Morvinski, Coby, and Yaniv Shani (2022). "Misaligned mindsets between borrowers and lenders
of small interpersonal loans." Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
169, 104117.
Newman, George E., and Daylian M. Cain (2014). "Tainted altruism: When doing some good is
evaluated as worse than doing no good at all." Psychological Science, 25(3), 648-655.
Oh, Catherine S., Jeremy N. Bailenson, and Gregory F. Welch (2018). "A systematic review of
social presence: Definition, antecedents, and implications." Frontiers in Robotics and AI
5: 114.
111
Olson, Jenny G., and Scott I. Rick (2021). "“You Spent How Much?” Toward an Understanding
of How Romantic Partners Respond to Each Other’s Financial Decisions." Current
Opinion in Psychology.
Ong, Qiyan, Walter Theseira, and Irene YH Ng (2019), "Reducing debt improves psychological
functioning and changes decision-making in the poor." Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 116 (15), 7244-7249.
Pandelaere, Mario, Barbara Briers, and Christophe Lembregts (2011). "How to make a 29%
increase look bigger: The unit effect in option comparisons." Journal of Consumer
Research, 38(2), 308-322.
Pelham, Brett W., Tin T. Sumarta, and Laura Myaskovsky (1994). "The easy path from many to
much: The numerosity heuristic." Cognitive Psychology, 26(2), 103-133.
Porter, Michael E., and Mark R. Kramer (2006). "The link between competitive advantage and
corporate social responsibility." Harvard Business Review, 84(12), 78-92.
Prelec, Drazen, and George Loewenstein (1998), “The red and the black: Mental accounting of
savings and debt,” Marketing science, 17(1), 4-28.
Rick, Scott I., Deborah A. Small, and Eli J. Finkel (2011), "Fatal (fiscal) attraction: Spendthrifts
and tightwads in marriage." Journal of Marketing Research, 48 (2), 228-237.
Ridgeway, Cecilia L (2014), "Why status matters for inequality." American Sociological
Review, 79 (1), 1-16.
112
Rona-Tas, Akos and Alya Guseva (2018), “Consumer Credit in Comparative Perspective,”
Annual Review of Sociology, 44(1), 5575.
Rose, Gregory M. and Linda M. Orr (2007), “Measuring and Exploring Symbolic Money
Meanings,” Psychology and Marketing, 24(9), 74361.
Ryu, Gangseog, and Jin K. Han (2009). "Word-of-mouth transmission in settings with multiple
opinions: The impact of other opinions on WOM likelihood and valence." Journal of
Consumer Psychology, 19(3), 403-415.
Schroeder, Juliana, Michael Kardas, and Nicholas Epley (2017), “The Humanizing Voice:
Speech Reveals, and Text Conceals, a More Thoughtful Mind in the Midst of
Disagreement,” Psychological Science, 28(12), 174562.
Sen, Sankar, and Chitra Bhanu Bhattacharya (2001). "Does doing good always lead to doing
better? Consumer reactions to corporate social responsibility." Journal of Marketing
Research, 38(2), 225-243.
Sharma, Eesha, Stephanie M. Tully, and Cynthia Cryder (2021), "Psychological Ownership of
(Borrowed) Money." Journal of Marketing Research, 58 (3), 497-514.
Short, John, Ederyn Williams, and Bruce Christie (1976). The Social Psychology of
Telecommunications. New York, NY: John Wiley.
Simmons, Joseph P., Leif D. Nelson, and Uri Simonsohn (2012), "A 21-word solution."
Available at SSRN 2160588.
113
Simpson, Jeffry A., Vladas Griskevicius, and Alexander J. Rothman (2012). "Consumer
decisions in relationships." Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22 (3), 304-314.
Skowronski, John J., and Donal E. Carlston (1987). "Social judgment and social memory: The
role of cue diagnosticity in negativity, positivity, and extremity biases." Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 52(4), 689.
Small, Deborah A., and Cynthia Cryder (2016). "Prosocial consumer behavior." Current Opinion
in Psychology, 10: 107-111.
Sparkman Jr, Richard M., and William B. Locander (1980). "Attribution theory and advertising
effectiveness." Journal of Consumer Research, 7(3), 219-224.
Spencer, Steven J., Mark P. Zanna, and Geoffrey T. Fong (2005), "Establishing a Causal Chain:
Why Experiments Are Often More Effective Than Mediational Analyses in Examining
Psychological Processes,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (6), 84551.
Sproull, Lee and Sara Kiesler (1986), “Reducing Social Context Cues: Electronic Mail in
Organizational Communication,” Management Science, 32(11), 14921512.
Sun, Katherine Qianwen and Michael L. Slepian (2020), The Conversations We Seek to
Avoid,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 160, 87105.
Sussman, Abigail B., and Eldar Shafir (2012). "On assets and debt in the psychology of
perceived wealth." Psychological science, 23 (1), 101-108.
114
Sussman, Abigail B., and Rourke L. O'brien (2016), "Knowing when to spend: Unintended
financial consequences of earmarking to encourage savings." Journal of Marketing
Research, 53 (5), 790-803.
Sweet, Elizabeth, Arijit Nandi, Emma K. Adam, and Thomas W. McDade (2013), "The high
price of debt: Household financial debt and its impact on mental and physical
health." Social Science & Medicine, 91, 94-100.
Torelli, Carlos J., Alokparna Basu Monga, and Andrew M. Kaikati (2012). "Doing poorly by
doing good: Corporate social responsibility and brand concepts." Journal of Consumer
Research, 38(5), 948-963.
Trachtman, Richard. (1999), “The money taboo: Its effects in everyday life and in the practice of
psychotherapy,” Clinical Social Work Journal, 27(3), 275-288.
Unger, Clive, Dhiraj Murthy, Amelia Acker, Ishank Arora, and Andy Chang (2020) “Examining
the evolution of mobile social payments in Venmo,” In International Conference on
Social Media and Society (pp. 101-110).
Valsesia, Francesca, and Kristin Diehl (2022). "Let me show you what I did versus what I have:
Sharing experiential versus material purchases alters authenticity and liking of social
media users." Journal of Consumer Research, 49(3), 430-449.
Walther, Joseph B. (1995), “Relational Aspects of Computer-Mediated Communication:
Experimental Observations over Time,” Organization Science, 6(2), 186203.
Walther, Joseph B. (1996), “Computer-Mediated Communication: Impersonal, Interpersonal,
and Hyperpersonal Interaction,” Communication Research, 23(1), 343.
115
Ward, Morgan K., and Susan M. Broniarczyk (2016), "Ask and you shall (not) receive: Close
friends prioritize relational signaling over recipient preferences in their gift
choices." Journal of Marketing Research, 53 (6), 1001-1018.
Wells, Gary L., and Paul D. Windschitl (1999). "Stimulus sampling and social psychological
experimentation." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25 (9), 1115-1125.
Wherry, Frederick F., Kristin S. Seefeldt, and Anthony S. Alvarez (2019), “To lend or not to
lend to friends and kin: Awkwardness, obfuscation, and negative reciprocity,” Social
Forces, 98(2), 753-793.
White, Katherine, and Darren W. Dahl (2006). "To be or not be? The influence of dissociative
reference groups on consumer preferences." Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16 (4)
404-414.
White, Katherine, and John Peloza (2009). "Self-benefit versus other-benefit marketing appeals:
Their effectiveness in generating charitable support." Journal of Marketing, 73 (4) 109-
124.
Wilcox, Keith and Andrew T. Stephen (2013), “Are Close Friends the Enemy? Online Social
Networks, Self-Esteem, and Self-Control,” Journal of Consumer Research, 40(1), 90
103.
Yamauchi, Kent T., and Donald J. Templer (1982), “The development of a money attitude
scale,” Journal of Personality Assessment, 46(5), 522-528.
116
Yoon, Yeosun, Zeynep Gürhan-Canli, and Norbert Schwarz (2006). "The effect of corporate
social responsibility (CSR) activities on companies with bad reputations." Journal of
Consumer Psychology, 16(4), 377-390.
Zelizer, Viviana A. (1989), “The social meaning of money:" special monies",” American Journal
of Sociology, 95(2), 342-377.
Zellermayer, Ofer (1996), "The pain of paying." unpublished dissertation, Department of Social
and Decision Sciences, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA.
Zhang, Y. Charles, and Norbert Schwarz (2012). "How and why 1 year differs from 365 days: A
conversational logic analysis of inferences from the granularity of quantitative
expressions." Journal of Consumer Research, 39(2), 248-259.
Zhang, Xinyi, Shiliang Tang, Yun Zhao, Gang Wang, Haitao Zheng, and Ben Y. Zhao (2017),
“Cold Hard E-Cash: Friends and Vendors in the Venmo Digital Payments System,” In
ICWSM (pp. 387-396).